The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticlesFull Access

The Role of Self-Awareness and Cognitive Dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease With and Without Impulse-Control Disorder

Abstract

The aim of this study was to investigate the clinical, neuropsychological, and self-awareness correlates of impulse-control disorder (ICD) in a group of 17 Parkinson’s disease (PD) subjects with an active ICD and a comparison group of 17 PD subjects without ICD. Self-awareness was assessed with the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale and patient–caregiver discrepancy scores from ratings on the Dysexecutive Questionnaire and the Everyday Memory Questionnaire–Revised. Self-awareness was comparable or increased in those with ICD, versus those without, and measures of neuropsychological functioning did not differ between the two groups. Those with ICD had more motor complications of PD therapy and were more likely to be on an antidepressant than those without ICD, whereas dopaminergic medication profiles were comparable between the two groups. In this group, PD patients with current ICDs were aware of their impulsivity. Although executive dysfunction may contribute to ICD behavior, it is not a necessary component. The awareness of the inability to resist these motivated behaviors may be a source of increased depression.

Impulse-control disorders (ICDs), characterized by a failure to resist an impulse, drive, or temptation to perform an act that is harmful to the person or others,1 have been increasingly recognized as a problem in Parkinson’s disease (PD). Cross-sectional prevalence for any ICD has been reported to be 14%, with roughly similar rates for each of four main types, including pathological gambling (PG), compulsive buying, compulsive sexual behaviors, and binge-eating behaviors, and more than one-fourth suffering from two or more ICDs.2 The behaviors involved in ICDs are thought to be due to dysfunctional dopaminergic stimulation in reward systems of the brain and have been likened to behavioral addictions.3 These disorders can have severe, negative consequences for PD patients and their families, as evidenced by published reports of ICD-related suicidality, marital discord, and financial ruin.4,5 Studies and clinical practice have implicated dopamine-agonist (DA) use as a main risk factor for development of ICDs in PD,2,6,7 although this is not exclusive, and a contribution of levodopa is not excluded.2 Other risk factors for ICDs include younger age, younger age at PD onset, being unmarried, a family history of gambling or alcohol problems, and a novelty-seeking personality,2,8 with some characteristics associated with particular ICD types, such as male gender in the case of PG and hypersexuality.9 Higher levels of depressive, anxiety, and obsessive-compulsive symptoms have also been reported in PD patients with ICDs.10,11

Studies of the neuro-cognitive characteristics associated with ICDs in PD have yielded mixed results. Executive dysfunction was shown in one study to be predictive of PG in a sample of PD patients without dementia,12 with deficits in set shifting and spatial planning,13 and working memory14 reported in PD patients with various ICDs compared with those without, consistent with studies of ICD in non-PD populations.15 In contrast, others have found a lack of difference in executive functioning8 and equal or even higher performance1618 in PD samples with ICD versus those without. Memory deficits have been reported in samples with hypersexuality and compulsive eating,13 but are generally thought to play less of a role than executive functions in ICD behaviors.

Impaired self-awareness or insight has been recognized as a feature of a number of neuropsychiatric disorders,19 including PD. PD patients have been shown to have impaired awareness in a range of pathologic behaviors, including cognitive deficits,20,21 psychiatric symptoms (including punding, a non goal-directed ICD)2228 and social dysfunction.20,26 However, self-awareness has not been examined in PD patients across the range of ICD types. The aim of this exploratory study was to investigate the correlations between clinical, neuropsychological, and self-awareness measures in PD patients with and without ICD. It was hypothesized that PD patients with current ICD (PD-ICD) would display greater deficits in self-awareness than PD patients without current ICD (No-ICD).

Methods

Participants

PD subjects identified as having an active ICD were recruited from consecutive patients with idiopathic PD (previously diagnosed by a movement disorders specialist or general neurologist by U.K. brain-bank criteria29) attending a tertiary movement disorders clinic (King’s College Hospital–London, U.K.) or from a sample recruited to an ongoing, randomized, controlled trial involving psychosocial treatment of ICDs (ICaDDS; research trial ethics reference: 08/H0807/1), who had agreed to be contacted for related research. A group of PD patients without known ICD, from the same clinics, was recruited for comparison without intention of matching the groups based on demographic or PD characteristics. Patients were identified by a specialist PD nurse and approached by one of the study researchers (JDM) via telephone or face-to-face at their clinic appointment. Exclusion criteria included Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE)30 <24 or physical, sensory, or communication deficits that would interfere with assessment. No patients approached for participation were excluded on the basis of these criteria. All were required to have a caregiver available and willing to participate. Caregivers were defined as a spouse, relative, or friend who was involved in their PD-related care on most days of the week. All approached patients agreed to participate, although one could not be reached later to schedule an assessment.

Current ICD, defined as meeting criteria for active ICD within the past month, was confirmed with a semistructured interview based on previously set criteria for pathologic/problem gambling,1 hypersexuality,6 compulsive shopping,31 and binge eating;1 17 patients meeting criteria for “current ICD” were evaluated, and 17 PD patients with “no current ICD” were evaluated for comparison. Two patients in the latter group had a history of impulsive behavior that did not meet a priori criteria for “current ICD.” Both of these participants’ impulse-control symptoms had been remitted for over 6 months at the time of assessment, based on review of their clinical history, and neither of them appeared to be outliers, as their scores were close to the mean of the group.

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their caregivers. This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committees for the participating institutions.

Assessments

Demographic, clinical, neuropsychological, and awareness information was gathered in a single session lasting approximately 90 minutes, which was completed in either the participant’s home or in the clinic. Breaks were provided as needed. PD patients were assessed while in the “on” medication state.

Clinical

All participants underwent a full examination of current motor symptoms, PD-related problems and treatment side effects, using the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, Sections I–IV,32 and Levodopa-Equivalent Daily Dose (LEDD) was calculated.33 Neuropsychiatric symptoms were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)34 and the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI).35 The administration of the NPI was modified to include combined information from both the caregiver and the patient.

Neuropsychological

Premorbid intellectual function was estimated with The National Adult Reading Test (NART).36 Registration, delayed recall, and delayed recognition were tested with the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised (HVLT–R).37 Attention and working memory were tested with the Digit Span task, forward and backward (total score out of 30). Sequencing ability, set-shifting, and visual search ability were assessed with the timed Trail-Making Test. Verbal Fluency was assessed for phonemic (F, A, S) and semantic (animals) word production in 1 minute per letter/category.

Patients and caregivers also independently completed corresponding versions of questionnaires to evaluate day-to-day manifestations of executive and memory problems. The Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX)38 examines difficulties related to impulsivity, apathy, distractibility, unconcern for social rules, and abstract thinking (Scored 0–80, with higher scores indicating more dysexecutive behavioral problems). The Everyday Memory QuestionnaireRevised (EMQ)39 measures memory failure in everyday life (scored out of 52, with higher scores indicating more memory problems).

Awareness

Self-awareness was assessed with the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale (BCIS),40 which consists of a Self-Reflectiveness subscale (9 items) and a Self-Certainty subscale (6 items, negatively scored). Higher scores indicate greater cognitive insight. Discrepancy scores were calculated for the DEX and the EMQ based on the difference between the participant and caregiver scores for corresponding versions of each instrument, such that a more negative Discrepancy score indicates greater unawareness (0 indicates full awareness). DEX Discrepancy scores can range from −80 to +80, and EMQ Discrepancy scores from −52 to +52.

Statistical Analysis

Group differences between PD-ICD and No-ICD were assessed by chi-square analysis in the case of categorical variables and independent-samples t-tests in the case of continuous variables. Pearson correlations were run between BDI scores and measures of awareness for the sample as a whole. Level of significance was set a priori at <0.05. Analyses were performed using SPSS Version 19 (IBM SPSS; 2010)

Results

The demographic and PD-related characteristics of participants completing assessment (N=34), broken down into PD-ICD and No-ICD groups, are shown in Table 1. Compared with No-ICD, PD-ICD were younger at the time of PD diagnosis and suffered more motor complications of PD therapy (UPDRS, part IV), whereas motor symptom severity (UPDRS, part III), dopamine agonist use, and LEDD were comparable between the two groups (although the total LEDD was nonsignificantly greater in the PD-ICD group; Table 1). In the PD-ICD group (N=17), 3 each met single criteria for pathological gambling and compulsive shopping, 2 each for compulsive eating and compulsive sexual activity, and 7 met criteria for two or more ICDs. All participants except one, who was a resident in a minimal-assistance retirement community, were living in their own homes at the time of the assessment. Caregivers (N=34) consisted of a 26 spouses, 5 adult children, 2 friends, and 1 sibling, with no significant differences in caregiver-type between the two groups.

TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Parkinson's Disease (PD) Groups With or Without Impulse Control Disorder (ICD)
PD-ICD (N=17)No-ICD (N=17)Test Statisticp
Age, years61.1 (7.5)63.8 (8.5)t[32] = −1.00NS
Gender, N, men/women11/68/9χ2=1.07NS
Age diagnosed, years48.1 (5.2)53.7 (10.0)t[32] = −2.070.047
PD duration13.1 (6.9)10.2 (5.6)t[32]=1.31NS
Side of onset, N (%)Right: 8 (47.1)Right: 9 (52.9)χ2=1.31NS
Dopamine-agonist use, N (%)10 (58.8)12 (70.6)χ2=0.52NS
L-dopa equivalent daily dose, mg1,677.9 (893.0)1,269.3 (560.7)t[32]=1.60NS
Hoehn & Yahr Stage2.8 (1.0)2.4 (1.3)t[32]=1.04NS
UPDRS Total78.0 (30.5)59.5 (27.7)t[32]=1.860.07
  I: Nonmotor Aspects of Daily Living17.2 (6.3)13.5 (6.2)t[32]=1.71NS
  II: Motor Aspects of Daily Living17.7 (9.9)12.8 (8.1)t[32]=1.57NS
  III: Motor Exam36.7 (16.1)28.5 (15.2)t[32]=1.52NS
  IV: Therapy Complications8.3 (5.0)4.9 (4.7)t[32]=2.050.049

Variables are mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise indicated.

UPDRS: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale.

TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Parkinson's Disease (PD) Groups With or Without Impulse Control Disorder (ICD)
Enlarge table

Neuropsychiatric Symptoms

PD-ICD and No-ICD did not differ significantly in terms of reported depressive symptoms (Table 2). On average, both groups reported depression symptoms in the mild range. Significantly more participants in PD-ICD than No-ICD were currently being treated with an antidepressant or antianxiety medication (58.8% versus 17.6%; χ2[1]=6.103; p=0.01). Individual NPI items showed no between group differences.

TABLE 2. Psychiatric, Neuropsychological, and Awareness Data for Parkinson's Disease (PD) Groups With or Without Impulse Control Disorder (ICD)
PD-ICD (N=17)No-ICD (N=17)Test Statisticp
NART–R108.2 (11.4) [N=16]108.8 (14.0)t[31] = −0.13NS
MMSE28.7 (1.2)28.5 (1.2)t[32]=0.43NS
NPI total (out of 144)15.8 (9.2)10.4 (8.6)t[32]=1.770.09
BDI14.7 (7.6)11.1 (6.3)t[32]=1.48NS
HVLT–R
  Total Recall22.6 (6.1)20.6 (5.3)t[32]=1.02NS
  Delayed Recall7.6 (3.5) [N=16]7.8 (2.9)t[31]= −0.18NS
  Recognition Discrimination10.8 (1.2) [N=16]10.4 (1.8)t[31]=0.74NS
Digit Span (out of 30)18.2 (4.9)16.8 (3.7)t[32]=0.95NS
Verbal Fluency
  Phonemic39.7 (16.7)47.4 (16.8)t[32] = −1.33NS
  Category15.8 (3.4)18.0 (4.9)t[32] = −1.5NS
Trail-Making Test
  Part A (s)61.1 (50.6)38.3 (15.9)t[32]=1.770.09
  Part B (s)122.9 (90.2)102.9 (55.9)t[32]=0.78NS
  B–A52.2 (44.9)64.8 (50.6)t[32] = −0.77NS
DEX
  Self28.9 (14.5)16.6 (8.2)t[32]=3.060.004
  Caregiver26.4 (14.2)19.7 (9.1)t[32]=1.66NS
  Discrepancy2.5 (13.9)−3.1 (11.1)t[32]=1.30NS
EMQ
  Self20.2 (12.5)9.8 (7.0)t[32]=3.000.005
  Caregiver12.3 (10.5)8.0 (5.1)t[32]=1.52NS
  Discrepancy8.5 (9.9)1.7 (5.3)t[32]=2.490.018
BCIS4.5 (4.2)0.31 (4.1) (N=16)t[31]=2.910.007
  Self-reflectiveness11.7 (3.3)9.3 (3.5)t[31]=2.020.05
  Self-certainty7.2 (3.3)8.9 (3.2)t[31] = −1.52NS

Variables are mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise indicated.

NART: National Adult Reading Test; MMSE: Mini-Mental State Exam; NPI: Neuropsychiatric Inventory; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; HVLT: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised; DEX: Dysexecutive Questionnaire; EMQ: Everyday Memory Questionnaire–Revised; BCIS: Beck Cognitive Insight Scale.

TABLE 2. Psychiatric, Neuropsychological, and Awareness Data for Parkinson's Disease (PD) Groups With or Without Impulse Control Disorder (ICD)
Enlarge table

Neuropsychological Testing and Awareness Measures

There were no significant differences between PD-ICD and No-ICD in premorbid IQ, screening MMSE score (range: 26–30), or performance on any of the neuropsychological tests of memory or executive function (Table 2).

Mean participant and caregiver ratings of everyday executive and memory function problems (DEX and EMQ, respectively) and corresponding participant–caregiver discrepancy scores are shown in Table 2. Self-rated executive functioning (DEX total score) was significantly lower in PD-ICD than in No-ICD, but total caregiver DEX score did not differ between the groups. Although the total DEX discrepancy score suggests greater awareness in the PD-ICD group, the difference was not significant (Table 2). Similar results were found in the DEX subscales (Figure 1) with none of the discrepancy scores reaching significance (Inhibition: t[32]=1.33; NS; Intentionality: t[32]=0.525; NS; Executive Memory: t[32]=1.95; p=0.06; Positive Affect: t[32]=1.08; NS; Negative Affect: t[32]=0.12; NS). Relevant to the issue of ICDs, the PD-ICD participants rated themselves significantly higher than No-ICD on the main DEX Impulsivity question (“Acts without thinking”) (t[32]=2.75; p=0.010). BCIS scores revealed greater overall Cognitive Insight and a trend toward greater Self-Reflectiveness in PD-ICD than in No-ICD, but no significant difference in the Self-Certainty subscore between groups (Table 2). EMQ self-ratings were significantly higher in PD-ICD than No-ICD, indicating more subjective memory complaints, with discrepancy scores reflecting a greater awareness of memory problems in the PD-ICD group (Table 2).

FIGURE 1. Mean Dysexecutive Questionnaire (DEX) Subfactor Scores by Self- and Caregiver Rating for Both the Parkinson’s Disease With ICD (PD-ICD) and Parkinson’s Disease Without ICD (No-ICD) Groups

a significant difference at p ≤0.05 for PD-ICD versus No-ICD self ratings.

b significant difference at p ≤0.01 for PD-ICD versus No-ICD self ratings.

Differences in caregiver ratings of PD-ICD and No-ICD did not reach significance for any of the subfactors.

For the sample as a whole (N=34), BDI scores were not significantly correlated with DEX Discrepancy score (r[32]=0.28; NS) or BCIS score (r[32]=0.16; NS), but were significantly correlated with EMQ Discrepancy score (r[32]=0.47; p=0.005); that is, greater depression, more memory problems.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first formal study to explore self-awareness of cognitive and behavioral issues in a sample of PD patients affected by the range of ICDs. The presence of ICDs was associated with awareness of impulsive behaviors and memory problems, as indexed by DEX and EMQ discrepancy scores, respectively, and greater cognitive insight into thoughts and behaviors on the BCIS, as compared with the No-ICD group. There was no difference in frontal executive dysfunction on neuropsychological testing between PD-ICD and No-ICD, a finding in line with some previous studies.13,14,18 Despite the lack of neuropsychological deficits as compared with No-ICD, PD-ICD participants self-reported significantly more everyday problems related to executive dysfunction, including impulsivity. These findings support the idea that PD patients with ICDs are less able to resist engaging in impulsive behaviors related to gambling, sex, eating, and shopping, despite recognizing their impulsivity.

In contrast to the present findings, impaired awareness of motor,4143 cognitive,20,21 psychiatric,2228 and social problems20,26 have been demonstrated in PD in previous studies. A model of such domain-specific impairments of awareness in neuropsychiatric disorders has been proposed, with the possibility of isolated impairments in particular domains.19 One proposed mechanism for such impairments in awareness relates to impaired ability to “shift set,” leading to a faulty appraisal of performance, poor recognition of failure, and a resultant lack of update of the patient’s “personal database.”44 This explanation appears applicable to ICDs in PD, given the characteristic inability to refrain from potentially harmful behaviors, despite repeated failures and negative consequences. However, the present study found an intact ability to “set-shift” in PD-ICD, as assessed by the TMT (Trail-Making Test) B–A score, although impairments on the same task have been previously reported to be associated with PD-ICD.13 Cilia et al.45 postulate that PD subjects with pathological gambling (PG) have impaired set-shifting after negative outcomes, and correlate this impairment with an anterior cingulate cortex–striatal disconnection in those with PG versus controls. Regarding our results, it may be that the TMT does not provide adequate risk–reward valence to examine set-shifting as it relates to ICD behaviors or is insufficiently demanding, since there are only two fixed ‘sets’ and shifts are predictable (cf.: the Wisconsin Card Sort test [WCST]). A recent study of altruistic punishment (punishing others to enforce social norms at a cost to oneself) in PD patients with ICD found that patients with ICD in the “on” medication state were more likely than controls to punish unfair investing behaviors carried out by others. Although not examining “set-shifting,” per se, this study highlights the idea that PD patients with ICDs may recognize problematic behaviors in others while lacking awareness of the problems caused by their own behaviors.46 This is relevant to ICDs in that being aware of impulse-control behaviors is not the same as recognizing them as problematic. This involves a different level of awareness in the domain of social cognition, an area that we did not specifically assess. Where the problem relates to its impact on others, social cognition may be critical and more important than executive functioning.

ICDs have consistently been associated with DA treatment,2,6,7 although it is clear that medications alone are not sufficient to cause ICD. In our sample, there was no significant difference between PD-ICD and No-ICD in the percentage of patients taking DAs, possibly because of increased vigilance among prescribers. Total LEDD was nonsignificantly higher in PD-ICD than in No-ICD. Overall, LEDDs were high as compared with those for PD samples on average, possibly resulting from a cohort of less-responsive cases being seen in the specialty movement disorders clinic from which we recruited participants. Voon et al.47 propose that differences in response to nonphysiologic dopaminergic stimulation are due to individual susceptibilities and differences in striatal function, and liken ICDs to a behavioral form of medication-related dyskinesias. Indeed, PD-ICD in our study also had more complications of PD therapy than No-ICD participants, despite similar LEDD across the two groups. Another recent study found a similar association between ICDs and motor fluctuations.48 Interestingly, Amanzio et al.42 examined awareness of dyskinesias in a PD sample and found high levels of unawareness while in the “on” state, with unawareness associated with set-shifting deficits on the WCST. They attributed this finding to levodopa’s detrimental effect on frontal-cingulate-subcortical circuits, which are felt to be important in awareness phenomena. Given similarities across disinhibitory pathologies, some authors have proposed that ICDs should be examined as part of a continuum of disinhibited movements, acts, and emotions affected by the basal ganglia, including levodopa-induced dyskinesias47 and other addictions,49 with the goal of elucidating mechanisms that will guide research and treatment.

Increased depressive and anxiety symptoms have been associated with ICDs in PD,10,11 although we found depression symptoms to be comparable in PD-ICD and No-ICD. It is generally thought that depressive symptoms increase as patients become more aware of their deficits or problems (and vice versa), and this has been shown in various neuropsychiatric populations.19 In our study, depression scores correlated with awareness of memory deficits, but not with awareness of executive functioning or cognitive insight. PD-ICD participants did not report more depressive symptoms than No-ICDs, despite intact awareness of their problems and higher self-ratings on DEX and EMQ. However, significantly more participants in the PD-ICD group were being treated with an antidepressant at the time of this study, indicating that PD-ICD patients may experience more depressive symptoms overall albeit remitted due to treatment. Furthermore, patients in remission may still maintain a depression-related cognitive style, including negative self-perceptions, which may explain overestimation of cognitive problems.

There are a number of limitations to consider in this study. First, our sample size was small and may not have provided adequate power to detect smaller differences across variables. However, the aim of this study was exploratory in nature and should serve as a basis for examining the relationship of neuropsychological factors and awareness in ICDs on a larger scale. Secondly, the validity of using patient–caregiver discrepancy methods to assess awareness depends on the caregiver as the “gold standard” for reporting patients’ problems or deficits. This may be problematic, as caregiver reports might be skewed by factors such as perceived caregiver burden, depression, or a lack of familiarity with the subtleties of patient deficits (i.e., having greater knowledge of the patients’ forgetting something than the patients themselves). However, this method has been used extensively in neuropsychiatric populations and consistently shows underestimation of deficits on behalf of patients in relation to caregivers. Studies specifically examining this issue in PD have shown good agreement of caregiver reports with clinician assessments on objective measures.20,21,50,51 Also, we used a questionnaire, the DEX, to query participants about dysexecutive problems in everyday life, including impulsivity, as a surrogate for impulsive behaviors in ICDs. The DEX picked up increased problems in the ICD group according to patient (and caregiver) ratings. However, this is not a substitute for scales assessing insight into ICD behaviors specifically and may limit the validity of the awareness findings. Incorporating ICD-specific instruments into future work will be important for drawing conclusions about the role of self-awareness in ICD behaviors. Last, we recruited participants from a tertiary movement disorders clinic population and some from a trial examining a psychosocial intervention for ICDs. Hence, this group is likely to have had PD-related problems, such as ICDs, brought to attention previously. Therefore, this group (both ICD and No-ICD) may be more aware of their PD-related problems than PD patients in the general population. This factor may also account, at least in part, for the young age at PD diagnosis in our sample. It should also be noted that we excluded patients with MMSE scores <24, and no participant scored <26; hence unselected populations of PD patients with ICDs may include those with generalized and specific executive deficits, which are likely to complicate their disorders.

Conclusions

In summary, PD patients suffering from ICDs are aware of their PD-related problems, including impulsivity. Executive dysfunction may contribute to ICD behaviors (and subsequent lack of awareness thereof), but is not a necessary component. The inability to resist these motivated behaviors may be a source of increased depression. Future treatments may utilize patients’ awareness of ICDs through adjunctive psychological approaches that focus on behavioral interventions in addition to adjusting medications. Although patient and clinical characteristics such as earlier age at onset and DA use are associated with ICDs, further research into ICDs as one form of disinhibitory pathology will be important to elucidate neural mechanisms and, ultimately, inform future management of these and other disinhibitory illnesses.

From the Dept. of Psychiatry, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland, OR; Dept. of Psychiatry and Northwest Parkinson's Disease Research, Education and Clinical Center, Portland Veterans Administration Medical Center, Portland, OR; Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK; Imperial Healthcare NHS Trust and West London Mental Health Trust; Dept. of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London, UK; Dept. of Mental Health & Specialist Care, Florence Nightingale School of Nursing & Midwifery, King's College London, London, UK; National Parkinson Foundation Centre of Excellence, Dept. of Neurology, King’s College Hospital, King’s Health Partners, London, UK; East Kent Hospitals University NHS Foundation Trust, William Harvey Hospital, Ashford, UK.
Correspondence: Joel Mack, M.D., Portland VA Medical Center, Portland, OR; e-mail:

Relevant conflicts of interest/financial disclosures: RGB has received honoraria for speaking at conferences and educational events from UCB Pharma, GSK, Lundbeck, and is an Advisory Board member for UCB Pharma. MS has received lecture fees from Medtronic, Inc., St. Judes, Inc., unrestricted educational grants from Ipsen and Solvay Pharmaceuticals, and sponsorship for educational meetings from Boehringer Ingelheim. KRC serves as the European Editor of Basal Ganglia, as an editorial board member of Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, and The Journal of Parkinson’s Disease, and, as the Liaison and PR Committee Chairman of the Movement Disorders Society, has received honoraria for academic lectures at sponsored symposiums from UCB Pharma, Britannia, GSK, Abbott, Teva, Medtronic, and Boehringer Ingelheim, and has received educational grants for research from UCB Pharma, Abbott, Boehringer Ingelheim, and Britannia.

Funding: The authors acknowledge support from: Parkinson’s UK; Authors RGB and ASD acknowledge support from the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College, London. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR, or the Department of Health.

References

1 American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000Google Scholar

2 Weintraub D, Koester J, Potenza MN, et al.: Impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease: a cross-sectional study of 3,090 patients. Arch Neurol 2010; 67:589–595Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Marks I: Behavioural (non-chemical) addictions. Br J Addict 1990; 85:1389–1394Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Smeding HM, Goudriaan AE, Foncke EM, et al.: Pathological gambling after bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation in Parkinson disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007; 78:517–519Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Merims D, Giladi N: Dopamine dysregulation syndrome, addiction, and behavioral changes in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2008; 14:273–280Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Voon V, Hassan K, Zurowski M, et al.: Prevalence of repetitive and reward-seeking behaviors in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006; 67:1254–1257Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Weintraub D, Siderowf AD, Potenza MN, et al.: Association of dopamine agonist use with impulse-control disorders in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2006; 63:969–973Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Voon V, Thomsen T, Miyasaki JM, et al.: Factors associated with dopaminergic drug-related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2007; 64:212–216Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Singh A, Kandimala G, Dewey RB, et al.: Risk factors for pathologic gambling and other compulsions among Parkinson’s disease patients taking dopamine agonists. J Clin Neurosci 2007; 14:1178–1181Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Pontone G, Williams JR, Bassett SS, et al.: Clinical features associated with impulse-control disorders in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006; 67:1258–1261Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Voon V, Sohr M, Lang AE, et al.: Impulse-control disorders in Parkinson disease: a multicenter case–control study. Ann Neurol 2011; 69:986–996Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Santangelo G, Vitale C, Trojano L, et al.: Cognitive dysfunctions and pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2009; 24:899–905Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Vitale C, Santangelo G, Trojano L, et al.: Comparative neuropsychological profile of pathological gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive eating in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2011; 26:830–836Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Djamshidian A, Jha A, O’Sullivan SS, et al.: Risk and learning in impulsive and nonimpulsive patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2010; 25:2203–2210Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, et al.: Neurocognitive functions in pathological gambling: a comparison with alcohol dependence, Tourette syndrome, and normal controls. Addiction 2006; 101:534–547Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Siri C, Cilia R, De Gaspari D, et al.: Cognitive status of patients with Parkinson’s disease and pathological gambling. J Neurol 2010; 257:247–252Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Djamshidian A, O’Sullivan SS, Lees A, et al.: Stroop Test performance in impulsive and non-impulsive patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2011; 17:212–214Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Shotbolt P, Moriarty J, Costello A, et al.: Relationships between deep brain stimulation and impulse-control disorders in Parkinson’s disease, with a literature review. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2012; 18:10–16Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Gilleen J, Greenwood K, David A: Lack of Insight and Awareness in Schizophrenia and Neuropsychiatric Disorders, in Neuropsychiatric Disorders, 1st Edition. Edited by Miyoshi KMorimura Y, Maeda K. Springer Publishing, 2010, pp 33–49CrossrefGoogle Scholar

20 Seltzer B, Vasterling JJ, Mathias CW, et al.: Clinical and neuropsychological correlates of impaired awareness of deficits in Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease: a comparative study. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2001; 14:122–129MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Sitek EJ, Sołtan W, Wieczorek D, et al.: Self-awareness of memory function in Parkinson’s disease in relation to mood and symptom severity. Aging Ment Health 2011; 15:150–156Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Aarsland D, Larsen JP, Cummins JL, et al.: Prevalence and clinical correlates of psychotic symptoms in Parkinson disease: a community-based study. Arch Neurol 1999; 56:595–601Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Barnes J, David AS: Visual hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: a review and phenomenological survey. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 70:727–733Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Holroyd S, Currie L, Wooten GF: Prospective study of hallucinations and delusions in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 70:734–738Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Goetz CG, Fan W, Leurgans S, et al.: The malignant course of “benign hallucinations” in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2006; 63:713–716Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 McKinlay A, Grace RC, Dalrymple-Alford JC, et al.: Neuropsychiatric problems in Parkinson’s disease: comparisons between self and caregiver report. Aging Ment Health 2008; 12:647–653Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Fasano A, Pettorruso M, Ricciardi L, et al.: Punding in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of patient’s awareness on diagnosis. Mov Disord 2010; 25:1297–1299Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Mack J, Rabins P, Anderson K, et al.: Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in a community-based Parkinson disease sample. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012; 20:123–132Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, et al.: Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992; 55:181–184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State:” a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 McElroy SL, Keck PE, Pope HG, et al.: Compulsive buying: a report of 20 cases. J Clin Psychiatry 1994; 55:242–248MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Fahn S, Elton R: Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, in Recent Developments in Parkinson's Disease Forum. Edited by Fahn SMarsden CGoldstein Met al.. NJ, McMillan Healthcare Information, 1987, pp 153–164Google Scholar

33 Tomlinson CL, Stowe R, Patel S, et al.: Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2010; 25:2649–2653Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, et al.: An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1961; 4:561–571Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Cummings JL: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: assessing psychopathology in dementia patients. Neurology 1997; 48(Suppl 6):S10–S16Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Nelson HE: The Revised National Adult Reading Scale (NARTR). Windsor, UK, Nelson, 1991Google Scholar

37 Brandt J, Benedict R: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised. Lutz, FL, Psychological Assessment Resources, 2001Google Scholar

38 Wilson BA: BADS: Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome. St Edmunds, UK, Thames Valley Test Company, 1996Google Scholar

39 Royle J, Lincoln NB: The Everyday Memory Questionnaire–Revised: development of a 13-item scale. Disabil Rehabil 2008; 30:114–121Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Beck AT, Baruch E, Balter JM, et al.: A new instrument for measuring insight: the Beck Cognitive Insight Scale. Schizophr Res 2004; 68:319–329Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Vitale C, Pellecchia MT, Grossi D, et al.: Unawareness of dyskinesias in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s diseases. Neurol Sci 2001; 22:105–106Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Amanzio M, Monteverdi S, Giordano A, et al.: Impaired awareness of movement disorders in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn 2010; 72:337–346Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Prigatano G, Maier F, Burns R: Anosognosia and Parkinson's disease, in The Study of Anosognosia, 1st Edition. Edited by Prigatano G. USA, Oxford University Press, 2010, pp 159–169Google Scholar

44 Mograbi DC, Brown RG, Morris RG: Anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease: the petrified self. Conscious Cogn 2009; 18:989–1003Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Cilia R, Cho SS, van Eimeren T, et al.: Pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson’s disease is associated with fronto-striatal disconnection: a path modeling analysis. Mov Disord 2011; 26:225–233Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Djamshidian A, O’Sullivan SS, Doherty K, et al.: Altruistic punishment in patients with Parkinson’s disease with and without impulsive behaviour. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49:103–107Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Voon V, Fernagut PO, Wickens J, et al.: Chronic dopaminergic stimulation in Parkinson’s disease: from dyskinesias to impulse-control disorders. Lancet Neurol 2009; 8:1140–1149Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Solla P, Cannas A, Floris GL, et al.: Behavioral, neuropsychiatric, and cognitive disorders in Parkinson’s disease patients with and without motor complications. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011; 35:1009–1013Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Okai D, Samuel M, Askey-Jones S, et al.: Impulse-control disorders and dopamine dysregulation in Parkinson’s disease: a broader conceptual framework. Eur J Neurol 2011; 18:1379–1383Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 McRae C, Diem G, Vo A, et al.: Reliability of measurements of patient health status: a comparison of physician, patient, and caregiver ratings. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2002; 8:187–192Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51 Martínez-Martín P, Benito-León J, Alonso F, et al.: Patients’, doctors’, and caregivers’ assessment of disability using the UPDRS-ADL section: are these ratings interchangeable? Mov Disord 2003; 18:985–992Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar