The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.10.4.426

Abstract

Relationships between measures of executive skills and neuropsychiatric and functional status were examined in a group of 31 patients with Alzheimer's disease. Deficits in four executive skills tests were significantly associated with the Agitation/Disinhibition factor score and Total Neuropsychiatric score on the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale, as well as the Activities subscore on the Blessed Dementia Scale. The majority of these associations remained significant after covariance for Mini-Mental State Examination scores. Executive dysfunction is associated with clinically relevant neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment in Alzheimer's disease. These associations may be independent of other cognitive deficits such as memory, language, and visuospatial skills, and may not be appreciated on routine clinical evaluations. Executive skills deficits, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and functional disability may emerge from shared neurobiological mechanisms.

Alzheimer's disease (AD) is characterized by impairment in multiple cognitive domains, including memory, language, visuospatial skills, and executive functions. Executive functions are those cognitive processes that orchestrate the performance of complex, goal-oriented tasks and behaviors.1 These abilities include motivation, strategy development and adjustment, response control, and abstraction, and they are mediated primarily by prefrontal brain regions.2,3 Impairment of executive functions occurs in multiple neuropsychiatric disorders, including dementias, schizophrenia, and major depression.46 The effect of AD on executive functions is poorly understood. Limited data suggest that executive dysfunction is common in AD2 and is associated with delusions,7 rapid progression of dementia,8 and a need for a high level of care.5

Agitation, psychosis, depression, and apathy are common neuropsychiatric disturbances in patients with AD. Such neuropsychiatric symptoms may result in increased caregiver distress,9 a higher rate of nursing home placement,10 and more rapid disease progression.11,12 Neuroimaging and neuropathologic studies indicate that neuropsychiatric symptoms are associated with dysfunction in specific brain regions and that dysfunction of the frontal cortex may be particularly relevant to noncognitive expressions of the illness.1316

There is little consensus on the relationship between cognitive deficits and the neuropsychiatric symptoms that occur in patients with AD; different studies have found no association,17 a positive correlation,18 weak positive associations,19,20 a negative association,21 and mixed relationships.22 Studies of cognitive deficits and mood symptoms have also revealed mixed results.2327 The relationship between cognitive impairment and functional disability is better supported.28,29 Most of these studies used measures of global cognition such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),30 which contains memory, language, and visuospatial items, but not tests of executive function. The relationship between executive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric or functional status remains unclear.

We evaluated the presence and the extent of executive dysfunction in patients with AD. Neuropsychiatric symptoms and overall functional ability were also measured. The goals of this study were to 1) test the hypothesis that executive dysfunction is associated with greater neuropsychiatric symptomatology and functional impairment, and 2) explore relationships between executive dysfunction and specific types of neuropsychiatric symptoms.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were recruited from the Dementia Clinic, Neurobehavior Inpatient Unit, and Geropsychiatry Inpatient Unit of the West Los Angeles Veterans Affairs Medical Center, and from the UCLA Memory Disorders Clinic and Alzheimer's Disease Clinic. A convenience sample of 31 patients was included in the study. Each patient and his or her closest relative consented to participate in the study after the procedure had been fully explained.

Each patient had undergone a thorough clinical evaluation that included complete blood count, chemistry panel, serum thyroid-stimulating hormone and vitamin B12 levels, and structural neuroimaging study with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) or computed tomography (CT). Cerebrospinal fluid analysis, electroencephalogram, urine heavy metal screen, and serum human immunodeficiency virus antibody assay were performed when clinically indicated. Final diagnosis was determined by a clinical research investigator.

Patients met the criteria for probable AD established by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association.31 Structural neuroimaging results for all patients with AD were normal, demonstrated cerebral atrophy, or showed only thin periventricular lucency (on CT) or hyperintensity (on MRI). Patients were excluded from the study if there was a history of a psychotic disorder unrelated to dementia, a history of head trauma resulting in loss of consciousness, a psychoactive substance use disorder, or a systemic illness or other neurological condition that could account for the cognitive impairment. The severity of noncognitive symptoms was not an inclusion or exclusion criterion. All patients were free of psychoactive medications for at least 3 weeks prior to the research assessment. No patient had been treated with a cholinesterase inhibitor.

Clinical Assessment

The neuropsychological assessment was completed by a subspecialty-trained neuropsychologist. We selected seven tests of executive function that we felt represented different aspects of executive functioning (shown in parentheses):

1.

Controlled Oral Word Association Test32 FAS subtest (verbal fluency, initiation).

2.

Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (MDRS)33 Conceptualization (similarities, abstraction).

3.

MDRS Initiation (verbal and motor sequences, patterns, initiation).

4.

Stroop34 Interference (response inhibition).

5.

Trail Making35 Part B (divided attention, sequencing).

6.

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)36 Categories (problem solving).

7.

WCST Perseverative Errors (set shifting and flexibility).

The assessment of neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional ability was performed by one investigator (D.L.S.) within 2 weeks of the neuropsychological assessment and included the following instruments:

1.

The MMSE, a measure of overall cognitive impairment.

2.

The Neurobehavioral Rating Scale (NRS), a 28-item observer-rated assessment of cognitive and noncognitive symptoms in dementia. Principal components analysis has revealed six NRS factors: Agitation/Disinhibition, Anxiety/Depression, Behavioral Retardation, Psychosis, Cognition/Insight, and Verbal Output Disturbance.27,37 The NRS Total Neuropsychiatric score represents the sum of 20 items measuring noncognitive symptoms. Each NRS item is scored on a scale of 0 (not present) to 6 (extremely severe).

3.

The Blessed Dementia Scale–Activities subscale (BDS-A),38 an 11-item caregiver-rated measure of the patient's ability to perform daily activities, including eating, dressing, managing money, and performing household tasks.

Statistical Analysis

We used Kendall's tau-b and Kendall's partial tau39 correlation analyses to examine the associations of executive dysfunction with neuropsychiatric symptomatology and with functional disability. Correlations independent of global cognitive impairment were examined by covarying for MMSE scores.

RESULTS

Patient Characteristics

Thirty-one men participated in this study; the majority of patients were recruited from VA treatment sites as described above. Mean age was 69.9 years, mean MMSE score was 17.6, mean duration of illness was 4.1 years, and mean educational background was 13.9 years.

Measures of Executive Function, Neuropsychiatric Symptoms, and Functional Ability

Scores on tests of executive function and scales of neuropsychiatric symptoms or functional ability are shown in Table 1. A broad range of scores was present on all measures. Moderate floor effects were observed on three executive skills tests: 13 patients (42%) were unable to perform the Stroop Interference Test (maximum time 500 seconds), 17 (55%) were unable to perform the Trail Making Test Part B (maximum time 500 seconds), and 10 (32%) committed the maximum 47 perseverative errors on the WCST. Data from these tests were excluded from further analysis. Higher scores on the remaining four tests of executive function indicate better performance. Higher scores on the NRS and the BDS-A indicate more severe neuropsychiatric symptoms or functional disability.

Relationships Between Executive Dysfunction and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms

Relationships between low scores on executive skills tests and greater neuropsychiatric symptoms emerged (Table 2). Executive dysfunction correlated most robustly with NRS Agitation/Disinhibition and Total Neuropsychiatric scores. Kendall's tau-b ranged from –0.31 to –0.55, reaching statistical significance at the P<0.01 level in all analyses. Statistically significant relationships also emerged between poor performance on some of the executive skills tests and scores on NRS Psychosis and Anxiety/Depression factors. There was no significant correlation between scores on executive skills tests and the NRS Behavioral Retardation score.

After covariance for MMSE using partial correlations, 8 of 11 correlations between executive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric symptoms remained statistically significant (tau-b range –0.32 to –0.45, P<0.05), indicating that the majority of these relationships were independent of the effects of MMSE scores (Table 3). MMSE scores did not correlate significantly with neuropsychiatric symptoms, with the exception of NRS Total Neuropsychiatric scores (tau-b=–0.32, P<0.01).

Relationships Between Executive Dysfunction and Functional Ability

Relationships were found between executive deficits and functional disability (tau-b range –0.33 to –0.70, P<0.005; Table 2). Correlations between functional impairment and three of four tests—MDRS Conceptualization, MDRS Initiation, and WCST Categories—maintained statistical significance after covariance for MMSE scores (P<0.05), despite a relatively high degree of correlation between MMSE and BDS-A scores (tau-b=–0.52, P<0.005).

DISCUSSION

In this study of patients with AD we found significant relationships between executive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric symptoms. Poor performance on four tests of executive functioning was significantly associated with greater degrees of agitated and disinhibited behaviors, as well as overall neuropsychiatric disturbance. Spearman correlation coefficients for these relationships ranged from –0.58 to –0.68, indicating that scores on executive skills tests accounted for 34% to 46% of the variance of the NRS Total Neuropsychiatric scores. Low scores on tests of executive function were also associated with psychosis and with anxiety and depression, although not for all tests and not as strongly. There was no association between executive dysfunction and scores reflecting apathy, blunted affect, and psychomotor retardation.

The majority of these relationships between executive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric symptoms were independent of MMSE scores, suggesting that there is greater specificity for executive deficits that extends beyond global cognitive abilities. Previous studies that examined the relationships between cognition and behavior in patients with AD did not focus on specific areas of cognition.1719,2126 Two studies compared the neuropsychological profiles of dementia patients with and without delusions. Jeste et al.7 reported that AD patients with delusions performed more poorly on tests of memory, conceptualization, and verbal fluency than those without delusions. Flynn et al.20 found that the delusional group had more difficulty with abstraction. Neither study controlled for degree of dementia or examined other types of neuropsychiatric symptoms. Our study demonstrates that a focused area of cognitive impairment, loss of executive skills, is accompanied by, and is a possible marker for, neuropsychiatric symptoms in AD, beyond the effects of global cognitive impairment.

Executive dysfunction was also significantly associated with inability to perform daily activities in this group of patients with AD. The relationship between WCST Categories scores and functional ability was particularly strong, and remained statistically significant after covariance with MMSE. The Spearman correlation and Spearman partial correlation coefficients for this relationship were –0.82 (P<0.005) and –0.58 (P<0.005), respectively, indicating that scores on this test accounted for more than 67% of the variance in scores of functional ability without controlling for MMSE, and 34% of the variance after controlling for MMSE. The ability to perform daily living activities to care for oneself may thus require a cognitive flexibility and a resistance to interference or distraction that are independent of overall level of cognition. The presence of executive skills deficits in a patient with AD therefore has important clinical implications for the level of care that the patient requires. Previous studies on AD, using other measures, have found relationships between cognition and functional impairment,28,29 although none, to our knowledge, has investigated specific areas of cognition in this regard. In a study of young, chronically ill schizophrenic inpatients and elderly residents in a retirement community, Royall et al.5 found that a measure of executive function was better correlated than MMSE scores with functional status in each group, suggesting that executive dysfunction has a substantial role in determining patients' level of functioning that is perhaps more important than global cognitive impairment, age, or disease.

Low executive skills scores were not associated with severity of all neuropsychiatric symptoms. Significant independent relationships were observed with agitated and disinhibited behaviors but did not appear with symptoms such as blunted affect and emotional withdrawal. These contrasting results suggest that executive deficits are not a proxy for generalized, overall morbidity, but that there is a specific connection between difficulty organizing and planning and the active, agitated, and disinhibited behaviors of AD. Such executive difficulties may have particular relevance to the ability to conform behaviors to socially appropriate norms. It is noteworthy that scores on the MDRS Conceptualization subtest, which measures the ability to recognize group similarities and differences and to appreciate metaphoric meaning, were associated with agitated behaviors and functional deficits. This finding suggests that difficulty with such “higher order” skills of abstract, inductive reasoning, which extend beyond the “organizational” executive skills such as fluency, planning, and strategy formation, is relevant to behavioral symptoms in patients with AD.

Underlying neurobiologic correlates may provide the basis for the relationships between executive dysfunction and specific neuropsychiatric symptoms. Both executive functioning and many neuropsychiatric disturbances are mediated through frontal-subcortical circuits.40 The dorsolateral prefrontal circuit facilitates executive functioning, and neuroimaging and neuropathologic studies implicate frontal cortical involvement in psychosis associated with AD.1416 Our previous results demonstrated a relationship between global frontal cortical hypometabolism and NRS total score, NRS Agitation/Disinhibition factor score, and Psychosis factor score in patients with AD, and a lack of association between frontal metabolic rate and the NRS Anxiety/Depression or Behavioral Retardation factor scores.16 Thus, deficits in executive skills, agitated behaviors, and functional disability in AD may share pathophysiologic processes in the frontal cortex, whereas apathy and blunted affect may not be associated with executive skills deficits and may be due to dysfunction outside the frontal lobe or to dysfunction in discrete subregions of the frontal cortex.

Other factors may be involved in the observed relationships between executive dysfunction and neuropsychiatric symptoms. Executive dysfunction may therefore be a marker of frontal lobe dysfunction but not etiologically related to neuropsychiatric phenomenology. The presence of neuropsychiatric disturbances could interfere with performance on executive skills tests. However, one would then expect behavioral retardation to be associated with initiation and cognitive flexibility. No such association was found. Nor was speed of performance relevant; the FAS test, which is timed, was the least associated of the tests with agitation, total neuropsychiatric symptomatology, and functional disability.

Other methodologic issues should be considered in interpreting the results of the study. The presence of small periventricular hyperintensities on MRI, which was not an exclusion criterion, may affect executive function.41 The neuropsychological tests selected depend on cognitive domains other than executive functioning, and therefore may not be “clean” measures of executive skills. (The WCST requires motor and visuospatial abilities; the FAS and MDRS tests call on language skills.) Conversely, the MMSE has been associated with executive functioning in AD42 and therefore may not be an ideal covariate to account for the effects of global cognitive impairment.

The results of this study suggest that agitated, disinhibited behaviors and deficits in self-care activities are associated with executive dysfunction in AD. In relation to noncognitive disturbances in AD, measurement of executive skills may be at least as important as measurement of global cognitive status. These findings have important clinical implications. Executive skills are not routinely assessed in patients with AD, although such assessment may assist the clinician in determining the patient's need for assisted living or psychiatric intervention. That three of the seven proposed executive skills tests could not be performed by 32% to 55% of a moderately impaired AD group suggests a need for better measures of executive skills in this population, as well as the possible utility of such measures in earlier detection of AD. Known neuroanatomic circuits and pathophysiologic observations support the relationships among frontal lobe dysfunction, deficits in executive skills, and certain neuropsychiatric symptoms. These data support the further study of executive dysfunction in AD, including the longitudinal course of deficits, associated neuropsychiatric disturbances, efficacy of treatment, and more precise elucidation of neurobiologic mechanisms.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This project was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH00910, National Institute on Aging Alzheimer's Disease Core Center Grant AG10123, a Department of Veterans Affairs Merit Review grant, and the American Psychiatric Association Program for Minority Research Training in Psychiatry.

TABLE 1.
TABLE 2.
TABLE 3.

Received September 15, 1997; revised November 10, 1997; accepted January 5, 1998. From the Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, UCLA School of Medicine, and the Psychiatry Service, Veterans Affairs Medical Center–West Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California. Address correspondence to Dr. Chen, UCLA Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, 760 Westwood Plaza C8-849, Los Angeles, CA 90024; e-mail:
References

1. Stuss DT, Benson DF (eds): The Frontal Lobes. New York, Raven, 1986Google Scholar

2. Sultzer DL: Mental Status Examination, in Textbook of Geriatric Neuropsychiatry, edited by Coffey CE, Cummings JL. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 111–127Google Scholar

3. Cummings JL, Coffey CE: Neurobiological basis of behavior, in Textbook of Geriatric Neuropsychiatry, edited by Coffey CE, Cummings JL. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994, pp 71–96Google Scholar

4. Binetti G, Magni E, Padovani A, et al: Executive dysfunction in early Alzheimer's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1996; 60:91–93Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Royall DR, Mahurin RK, True JE, et al: Executive impairment among the functionally dependent: comparisons between schizophrenic and elderly subjects. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1813–1819Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Jones BP, Henderson M, Welch CA: Executive functions in unipolar depression before and after electroconvulsive therapy. Int J Neurosci 1988; 38:287–297Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Jeste DV, Wragg RE, Salmon DP, et al: Cognitive deficits of patients with Alzheimer's disease with and without delusions. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 149:184–189Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Mann UM, Mohr E, Gearing M, et al: Heterogeneity in Alzheimer's disease: progression rate segregated by distinct neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic profiles. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992; 55:956–959Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Drinka TJK, Smith JC, Drinka PJ: Correlates of depression and burden for informal caregivers of patients in a geriatrics referral clinic. J Am Geriatr Soc 1987; 35:522–525Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Steele C, Rovner B, Chase GA, et al: Neuropsychiatric symptoms and nursing home placement of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:1049–1051Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Mortimer JA, Ebbitt B, Jun S-P: Predictors of cognitive and functional progression in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1992; 42:1689–1696Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Stern Y, Mayeux R, Sano M, et al: Predictors of disease course in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1987; 37:1649–1653Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Kotrla KJ, Chacko RC, Harper RG, et al: SPECT findings on psychosis in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1470–1475Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Grady CL, Haxby JV, Schapiro MB, et al: Subgroups in dementia of the Alzheimer type identified using positron emission tomography. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1990; 2:373–384LinkGoogle Scholar

15. Zubenko GS, Moossy J, Martinez AJ, et al: Neuropathologic and neurochemical correlates of psychosis in primary dementia. Arch Neurol 1991; 48:619–624Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Sultzer DL, Mahler ME, Mandelkern MA, et al: The relationship between neuropsychiatric symptoms and regional cortical metabolism in Alzheimer's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995; 7:476–484LinkGoogle Scholar

17. Teri L, Larson EB, Reifler BV: Behavioral disturbance in dementia of the Alzheimer's type. J Am Geriatr Soc 1988; 36:1–6Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Swearer JM, Drachman DA, O'Donnell BF, et al: Troublesome and disruptive behaviors in dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 1988; 36:784–790Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Cooper JK, Mungas D, Weiler PG: Relation of cognitive status and abnormal behaviors in Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990; 38:867–870Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20. Flynn FG, Cummings JL, Gornbein J: Delusions in dementia syndromes: investigation of behavioral and neuropsychological correlates. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1991; 3:364–370LinkGoogle Scholar

21. Cummings JL, Miller B, Hill MA, et al: Neuropsychiatric aspects of multi-infarct dementia and dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol 1987; 44:389–393Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Reisberg B, Franssen E, Sclan S, et al: Stage specific incidence of potentially remediable behavioral symptoms in aging and Alzheimer disease: a study of 120 patients using the BEHAVE-AD. Bull Clin Neurosci 1989; 54:95–112Google Scholar

23. Pearson JL, Teri L, Reifler BV, et al: Functional status and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's patients with and without depression. J Am Geriatr Soc 1989; 37:1117–1121Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Rovner BW, Broadhead J, Spencer M, et al: Depression and Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry 1989; 146:350–353Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Breen AR, Larson EB, Reifler BV, et al: Cognitive performance and functional competence in coexisting dementia and depression. J Am Geriatr Soc 1984; 32:132–137Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Fischer P, Simanyi M, Danielczyk W: Depression in dementia of the Alzheimer type and in multi-infarct dementia. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:1484–1487Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Sultzer DL, Levin HS, Mahler ME, et al: Assessment of cognitive, psychiatric, and behavioral disturbances in patients with dementia: the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale. J Am Geriatr Soc 1992; 40:549–555Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Ford GR, Haley WE, Thrower SL, et al: Utility of Mini-Mental State Exam scores in predicting functional impairment among white and African American dementia patients. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 1996; 51:M185–188Google Scholar

29. Auer SR, Sclan SG, Yaffee RA, et al: The neglected half of Alzheimer disease: cognitive and functional concomitants of severe dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 1994; 42:1266–1272Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh P: “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 1984; 34:939–944Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Benton AL, Hamsher KdeS (eds): Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA, University of Iowa, 1976Google Scholar

33. Mattis S: Mental status examination for organic mental syndrome in the elderly patient, in Geriatric Psychiatry, edited by Bellak L, Karasu TB. New York, Grune and Stratton, 1976Google Scholar

34. Stroop JR: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18:643–662CrossrefGoogle Scholar

35. Reitan RM, Tarshes EL: Differential effects of lateralized brain lesions on the Trail Making Test. J Nerv Ment Dis 1959; 129:257–262Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Nelson HE: A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 1976; 12:313–324Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Sultzer DL, Berisford MA, Gunay I: The Neurobehavioral Rating Scale: reliability in patients with dementia. J Psychiatr Res 1995; 29:185–191Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Davis PB, Morris JC, Grant E: Brief screening tests versus clinical staging in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Am Geriatr Soc 1990; 38:129–135Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Kendall MG (ed): Rank Correlation Methods, 5th edition. New York, Oxford University Press, 1990Google Scholar

40. Cummings JL: Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol 1993; 50:873–880Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41. Boone KB, Miller BL, Lesser IM, et al: Neuropsychological correlates of white-matter lesions in healthy elderly subjects: a threshold effect. Arch Neurol 1992; 49:549–554Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Eberling JL, Reed BR, Baker MG, et al: Cognitive correlates of regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer's disease. Arch Neurol 1993; 50:761–766Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar