The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

A pathy—a lack of motivation, interest, or initiation—has received much attention in neuropsychiatry. In this context, apathy has been conceptualized as the behavioral and cognitive result of a neurological lesion (such as a frontal or frontal-subcortical lesion). Cross-sectional studies have found clinically significant apathy symptoms in one-quarter to one-half of elderly patients with neurological illnesses such as Alzheimer disease, 13 extrapyramidal diseases, 45 and stroke, 6 compared with a prevalence of 1%–2% in the elderly community. 7 As reviewed elsewhere, studies have found that apathy in neuropsychiatric disease is associated with greater functional impairment, depressive symptomatology, and cognitive dysfunction. 89

A frequent clinical debate of “is it depression or is it apathy?” has the goal of contrasting depression, a treatable condition, with apathy, conceptualized as a fixed deficit. 4 , 911 However, apathy could also arise after a severe stressor. Disabling medical events, such as a hip fracture, produce not only psychological stress but also immobility and pain. The range of reactions to such stress includes a decrease in goal-directed behavior as well as dysphoria. 12

Disabling medical events, then, could lead to apathy syndromes in elderly persons in the same way that depression frequently develops in this context. 13 Apathy is a potentially critical variable in this population, which faces intensive rehabilitation and other challenges to recovery, as it predicts poorer rehabilitation participation and poorer functional recovery in geriatric rehabilitation. 14 If apathy in this context is subject to change, it could be a target for treatment. However, no research has examined changes in apathy after a disabling medical event.

In the current study, we used the Apathy Evaluation Scale, 15 an interview-based measure of apathy symptoms and signs, to assess apathy prospectively in elderly hip fracture patients. We hypothesized that apathy symptoms would predict functional recovery, and that those with initially high but improved apathy symptoms would have better functional recovery than those with persistently elevated apathy.

METHODS

Sample Recruitment

The study recruited consecutive hip fracture admissions to an acute care hospital from March 2002 to October 2004, as described previously. 16 Participants received surgical repair and were ages 60 or older, able to provide informed consent, free of metastatic cancer, and had a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) 17 score greater than 15. This study was approved by the university’s institutional review board.

Measures

Participants were prospectively observed for 6 months. They were assessed at the end of their acute hospital stay (i.e., at baseline) and then 2 weeks later with the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) 15 to measure signs and symptoms of apathy. The AES is a comprehensive measure of the observable behavioral, cognitive, and emotional concomitants of goal-directed behavior; it has 18 items and scores range from 18 to 72, with higher scores indicating more apathy. It measures how a subject feels at a particular time (rather than functioning as a dispositional or trait-like measure) and therefore can be used to examine changes in apathy over time. Cronbach alpha for the measure in this study was 0.93. Participants were also assessed for depressive symptoms with the 17-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) 18 and the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. 1920 Cognitive functioning was measured with the MMSE, the initiation/perseveration subscale of the Dementia Rating Scale, 21 the Trail Making test parts A and B, 22 and the logical memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd ed. 23 Delirium was measured with the Delirium Rating Scale. 24 Additionally, medical comorbidity was measured using the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale for Geriatrics. 25 This scale has items (scored from 0 to 4) for different domains of medical illness; for example, those with neurological illness (e.g., prior stroke) would have a positive score on the neurological item (#12).

Functional status was measured at baseline and at 2, 12, and 26 weeks later, using the 13 motor items of the Functional Independence Measure (FIM-motor). 26 This instrument rates dependency on other persons or assistive devices for activities of daily living and mobility tasks and is scored on a range from 13 (complete dependency for all activities of daily living and mobility tasks) to 91 (complete independence). For those who received rehabilitation in a skilled nursing facility or inpatient rehabilitation hospital, we measured their participation in physical and occupational therapy using the Pittsburgh Rehabilitation Participation Scale (PRPS), 27 a validated scale of patient participation in inpatient rehabilitation which provides a clinician-measured summary score (1–6, with higher scores indicating better participation) for each therapy session.

Analysis

The first hypothesis was that baseline apathy symptoms predicted functional recovery. Thus we examined (1) distribution of baseline AES scores and rate of clinically significant apathy symptoms, (2) demographic and clinical characteristics associated with apathy symptoms using Pearson correlation coefficients, and (3) baseline apathy symptoms as a predictor of trajectory of FIM-motor scores over 6 months, using a mixed effect repeated measures analysis.

Our second hypothesis was that improved apathy was associated with improved functional recovery. Thus we (1) examined distribution of changes from baseline to week 2 among initially high-apathy participants; (2) compared functional recovery between patients grouped by high versus low baseline and follow-up levels of apathy symptoms with persistently high AES scores (“high-high”) and persistently low-apathy patients using a mixed effect repeated measures model; and (3) examined correlates of improved apathy among cognition, depression, and other variables using Pearson correlation coefficients and t tests.

RESULTS

Study Group

We approached 141 patients; 13 refused the study (or dropped out before providing baseline data) and two were excluded from further participation because of moderate to severe cognitive impairment. Thus, the study group comprised 126 patients. Baseline and week 2 characteristics of the participants are summarized in Table 1 .

TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the 126 Participants
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the 126 Participants
Enlarge table

Rates of Clinically Significant Apathy Symptoms

The mean AES score was 33.7 (SD=9.4) at baseline and 33.4 (SD=9.6) at week 2. Examining the distributions found no natural cutoff; therefore, we chose the cutoff of ≥38 as a definition of clinically significant apathy symptoms, as previously reported. 15 By this cutoff, 46/126 patients (37%) had clinically significant apathy at baseline and 36/113 patients (32%) had clinically significant apathy at week 2.

Correlates of Apathy Symptoms

Next we examined the correlation of baseline Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) scores with age and baseline clinical characteristics. Of the two depression measures, AES scores were significantly correlated (p<0.001) with both the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia (r=0.42, n=90) and HAM-D scores (r=0.40, n=126). Among measures of cognitive function, AES scores were significantly correlated (p<0.001) with general cognitive ability as based on the MMSE (r=−0.42, n=126) as well as measures of executive function (Delirium Rating Scale initiation/perseveration subscale, r=−0.57, n=82 and Trails B, r=0.51, n=88) and attention/psychomotor speed (Trails A, r=0.49, n=92), but not delayed recall (Logical Memory Test, r=−0.12, n=108). Scores on the AES scores also correlated (p<0.001) with Delirium Rating Scale scores (r=0.37, n=126), postfracture FIM-motor scores (r=−0.38, n=126), and age (r=0.27, n=126). Scores on the AES were not significantly associated with overall medical burden (Cumulative Illness Rating Scale for Geriatrics score, r=0.14, n=121) or neurological illness (Cumulative Illness Rating Scale for Geriatrics item 12, r=0.14, n=125). At baseline, 41 patients (33%) were taking sedative medications, and 64 patients (52%) were taking opiate pain medications; apathy scores were not different in those taking these medications versus those without, nor were these medications associated with functional recovery (data not shown).

For depression and delirium measures we examined correlations with and without items in those scales that overlapped with the apathy construct, consistent with prior literature. 6 Thus, we removed three items from the HAM-D (item 7, “work and activities,” item 8, “psychomotor retardation,” and item 13, “somatic symptoms general”), four items (items 3, 6, 8, and 11) from the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia, and one item (item 9, “apathy”) from the Delirium Rating Scale. However, removing these items did not appreciably change either the magnitude or significance of the correlations of AES with these measures (data not shown).

Hypothesis 1: Apathy Symptoms Predict Functional Recovery

Table 2 shows the results of a series of univariate mixed-effects models predicting functional recovery from the baseline (immediate postfracture) period to 26 weeks. AES scores predicted recovery, with a moderate effect size, while depression measures were not significant predictors. In a multivariate model including all covariates that were potential predictors of recovery ( Table 3 ), AES scores remained a significant predictor of functional recovery, albeit with reduced effect size. Comparing high baseline apathy (AES >38) with low apathy groups in a mixed effects model found similar results; apathy group was a significant predictor (F=20.7, df=1, 124, p<0.001), with FIM-motor improvement at 2 weeks of 8.9 versus 27.6 in the high versus low groups, respectively. By 26 weeks, the FIM-motor improvement was 17.7 versus 31.4 in the high versus low groups, respectively.

TABLE 2. Univariate Models of Putative Baseline Predictors of FIM-Motor Changes
TABLE 2. Univariate Models of Putative Baseline Predictors of FIM-Motor Changes
Enlarge table
TABLE 3. Multivariate Model Including All Predictors of FIM-Motor Changes
TABLE 3. Multivariate Model Including All Predictors of FIM-Motor Changes
Enlarge table

Because these findings supported our hypothesis that baseline apathy symptoms predicted functional recovery, we then examined whether rehabilitation participation may account for the association of AES scores with functional recovery, as prior research has found that apathy predicts participation in rehabilitation 14 and that rehabilitation participation predicts functional outcome. 28 First, we confirmed that AES scores correlated with Pittsburgh Rehabilitation Participation Scale (PRPS) scores: baseline AES was correlated with mean PRPS scores in occupational therapy (r=−0.39, p<0.001, n=97) and physical therapy (r=−0.29, p<0.01, n=103). Next, we added mean PRPS scores to the multivariate model presented in Table 3 . While PRPS scores predicted poorer functional outcome in this revised model (F=8.96, df=1, 96, p=0.0035, effect size=0.27), the effect size of the association of AES with functional outcome did not change. Finally, we examined a reduced model that included AES, PRPS, and an AES × PRPS interaction term; this term was trend-level significant (F=3.2, df=1, 104, p=0.08) while AES was not significant (F=0.6, df=1, 104, p=0.46). Ensuing correlations showed a significant relationship between PRPS and FIM-motor outcome in individuals with AES score ≥38, and no relationship among those with AES score <38.

Changes in Apathy

To examine changes in apathy from baseline to week 2, we divided the sample into four groups: those with high AES scores (≥38) at both time points (“high-high” or persistently high apathy, n=25), those with high baseline AES scores but with lower scores (<38) at week 2 (“high-low” or improved apathy, n=13), those with low AES scores (<38) at both time points (“low-low” or persistently low apathy, n=64), and those with initially low AES scores but high scores at week 2 (“low-high,” n=11). We grouped participants in this way to provide clinically salient observations. The improved apathy group had a mean (SD) change of −8.2 (4.5) in AES scores, corresponding to a 19% decline, from baseline to week 2, while the persistent apathy group had a change of 1.6 (5.7), corresponding to a 4% increase in scores.

Hypothesis 2: Improved Apathy is Associated With Better Functional Outcome Than Persistent Apathy

Figure 1 shows the trajectory of FIM-motor scores in these four groups. The main interest was to contrast those with improved (“high-low”) apathy with those with persistent (“high-high”) apathy. As the figure shows, individuals with improved apathy had a better functional recovery; however, the modeled data are suggestive of an effect early but not late in recovery. Therefore we ran post-hoc tests comparing the groups in terms of FIM-motor improvements at 2 weeks, 12 weeks, and 26 weeks. As this figure shows, those with initially high but improved apathy symptoms showed significantly better functional recovery at 2 weeks, but not at 12 or 26 weeks, compared to those with persistently high apathy symptoms.

FIGURE 1: FIM-Motor Changes in Participants With Initially High but Improved Apathy Scores (“High-Low”), Compared With Persistently High Apathy, Persistently Low Apathy, or Initially Low but Worsened Apathy Scores (“Low-High”)

Those with persistently high apathy had lower improvement in FIM-motor, compared with all other groups, at 2 weeks (p<0.05). There were no group differences by 26 weeks.

Correlates of Improved Apathy

Because these findings supported our hypothesis that improved apathy symptoms were associated with better functional recovery than persistent symptoms, we then examined clinical and demographic correlates of improved apathy. Delirium scores were lower and MMSE scores higher at both baseline and week 2 for those with improved apathy than for those with persistently high apathy; no other demographic or clinical features (including neurological illness, benzodiazepine use, or opiate use) differed significantly between these two groups. Baseline delirium scores and MMSE for the “high-low” and “high-high” apathy groups were 4.0 (SD=2.0) versus 8.6 (SD=5.6) and 24.9 (SD=2.8) versus 22.1 (SD=3.7), respectively. Adding these terms to the model did not change the strength of the difference between improved and persistent apathy symptoms in terms of functional outcomes (data not shown).

We also examined whether AES scores in the overall sample changed in concert with cognitive or depressive symptom scores between baseline and week 2. However, we found small and nonsignificant correlations of change in AES score with change in MMSE score (r=0.14, n=109), change in Delirium Rating Scale score (r=0.13, n=111), and change in HAM-D score (r=−0.08, n=112). Thus, changes in apathy symptoms between baseline and week 2 were independent of changes in cognition, delirium, or depressive symptoms.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we found a high rate (37%) of clinically significant symptoms of apathy in elderly persons after a hip fracture, comparable to rates in samples of elderly patients with neurological disease and considerably higher than a prevalence of 1%–2% in community elderly. 7 Supporting our first hypothesis, baseline apathy scores predicted functional recovery in the 26 weeks after hip fracture; poorer participation in physical and occupational therapy in those with high apathy scores may account in part for the poorer functional outcome. Additionally, older age and more cognitive impairment at baseline predicted poorer functional recovery. Apathy symptoms correlated moderately with delirium, depression, and cognitive measures, commensurate with prior research. 15 We also found that, among those with initially high apathy symptoms, by 2 weeks later one-third had improved.

Supporting our second hypothesis, improved apathy symptoms were associated with a better functional recovery. Patients with lower levels of delirium symptoms and better cognitive function were more likely to demonstrate improvement in apathy over the first 2 weeks. The difference between improved and persistent apathy groups in terms of functional outcome was no longer apparent by 12 or 26 weeks postfracture; this may reflect that improvements in apathy in the acute postfracture period have only an acute effect on function (i.e., during rehabilitative efforts). For example, later events, such as a medical incident or psychosocial problems, could have obscured the relationship between these acute improvements in apathy and long-term functional outcome.

These findings are important for clinicians treating elderly patients who have suffered disabling medical events. In this setting, apathy symptoms are common and potentially diminish over a relatively short time. This point is important because if apathy can improve, leading to better functional recovery, then apathy may be an important potential target for detection and intervention in the medical setting. 29 Published interventions for apathy have included stimulants and cholinesterase inhibitors, though these agents appear to have modest efficacy. 9 Another potential treatment in the context examined here (acute disabling medical events) is medical rehabilitation, providing an antiapathy effect similar to that posited for the apparent antidepressant effect of rehabilitation in elderly persons. 3031 The high-intensity activity in this intervention may have an inherent antiapathy effect, 32 and successful rehabilitation alleviates functional disability and thus improves sense of control. 33

One important limitation was our lack of brain imaging in this sample. In our study, the association of apathy with measures of attention and executive function but not memory suggests a prefrontal cortical association, as suggested by some 34 but not all 35 studies of apathy and neuropsychological function in the context of depression. Neuroimaging will be a critical part of future research delineating the neurobiological underpinnings of apathy following disabling medical events.

Another important limitation of the sample is its naturalistic nature, which prevents us from causally connecting apathy with subsequent functional outcome or understanding why some initially apathetic individuals improved (possibilities include the effect of rehabilitative efforts or the passage of time—regression to the mean). Thus, future research on apathy should further examine its persistence over time and should include clinical trials to treat apathy. It would be appropriate for clinical trials of pharmacologic and nonpharmacologic treatments in disabled medically ill elderly (not only those with neurological conditions) to systematically test the effects of various interventions on apathy and resultant benefits for functional recovery and other quality of life outcomes. A final limitation of the current study is that we did not have a prefracture assessment of apathy, which would be desirable (though difficult in terms of feasibility) for studies of apathy arising in the context of medical events.

In summary, we found a high rate of apathy symptoms in this non-neurological sample of acutely disabled medically ill elderly. Apathy symptoms appeared to have a negative impact on functional recovery. Improvement in apathy symptoms in a significant minority of patients, with concomitant improved functional recovery, generates the hypothesis that apathy symptoms could be a target of treatment with the goal of improving functional outcomes after disabling medical events.

Received November 26, 2007; revised February 11 and June 20, 2008; accepted June 24, 2008. Dr. Lenze is affiliated with the Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine; Dr. Munin is affiliated with the Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine; Drs. Dew, Marin, Butters, Whyte, Begley, and Reynolds are affiliated with the Advanced Center for Interventions and Services Research in Late Life Mood Disorders and John A. Hartford Center of Excellence in Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine; Dr. Skidmore is affiliated with the Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Pittsburgh. Address correspondence to Dr. Lenze, 660 S Euclid Ave. Box 8134, St. Louis, MO 63110; [email protected] (e-mail).

Copyright © 2009 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

References

1. Lyketsos CG, Steinberg M, Tschanz JT, et al: Mental and behavioral disturbances in dementia: findings from the Cache Country Study on Memory in Aging. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:708–714Google Scholar

2. Aarsland D, Cummings JL, Larsen JP, et al: Neuropsychiatric differences between Parkinson’s disease with dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001; 16:184–191Google Scholar

3. Clark DE, van Reekum R, Simard M, et al: Apathy in dementia: an examination of the psychometric properties of the apathy evaluation scale. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007; 19:57–64Google Scholar

4. Kirsch-Darrow L, Fernandez HF, Marsiske M, et al: Dissociating apathy and depression in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 2006; 67:33–38Google Scholar

5. Dujardin K, Sockeel P, Devos D, et al: Characteristics of apathy in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2007; 22:778–784Google Scholar

6. Brodaty H, Sachdev PS, Withall A, et al: Frequency and clinical, neuropsychological and neuroimaging correlates of apathy following stroke: the Sydney Stroke Study. Psychol Med 2005; 35:1–10Google Scholar

7. Onyike CU, Sheppard JE, Tschanz JT, et al: Epidemiology of apathy in older adults: the Cache County study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007; 15:365–375Google Scholar

8. Levy ML, Cummings JL, Fairbanks LA, et al: Apathy is not depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1998; 10:314–319Google Scholar

9. van Reekum R, Stuff DT, Ostrander L: Apathy: why care? J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005; 17:7–19Google Scholar

10. Starkstein SE, Ingram L, Garau ML, et al: On the overlap between apathy and depression in dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005; 76:1070–1074Google Scholar

11. Weitzner MA, Kanfer S, Booth-Jones M: Apathy and pituitary disease: it has nothing to do with depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005; 17:159–166Google Scholar

12. Shumake J, Gonzalez-Lima F: Brain systems underlying susceptibility to helplessness and depression. Behav Cogn Neurosci 2003; 2:198–221Google Scholar

13. Lenze EJ, Rogers JC, Martire LM, et al: The association of late-life depression and anxiety with physical disability: a review of the literature and prospectus for future research. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001; 9:113–135Google Scholar

14. Resnick B, Zimmerman SI, Magaziner J, et al: Use of the Apathy Evaluation Scale as a measure of motivation in elderly people. Rehabil Nurs 1998; 23:141–147Google Scholar

15. Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S: Reliability and validity of the Apathy Evaluation Scale. Psychiatry Res 1991; 38:143–162Google Scholar

16. Lenze EJ, Munin MC, Skidmore ER, et al: Onset of depression in elderly persons after hip fracture: implications for prevention and early intervention of late-life depression. J Am Geriatr Soc 2007; 55:81–86Google Scholar

17. Folstein MF, Folstein SW, McHugh PR: “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Google Scholar

18. Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62Google Scholar

19. Alexopoulos GS, Abrams RC, Young RC, et al: Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. Biol Psychiatry 1988; 23:271–284Google Scholar

20. Alexopoulos GS, Abrams RC, Young RC, et al: Use of the Cornell scale in nondemented patients. J Am Geriatr Soc 1988; 36:230–236Google Scholar

21. Mattis S: Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1988Google Scholar

22. Reitan R, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test. Tucson, Neuropsychology Press, 1985Google Scholar

23. Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1997Google Scholar

24. Trzepacz PT, Baker RW, Greenhouse J: A symptom rating scale for delirium. Psychiatry Res 1988; 23:89–97Google Scholar

25. Miller MD, Paradis CF, Houck PR, et al: Rating chronic medical illness burden in geropsychiatric practice and research: application of the Cumulative Illness Rating Scale. Psychiatry Res 1992; 41:237–248Google Scholar

26. Linacre JM, Heinemann AW, Wright BD, et al: The structure and stability of the Functional Independence Measure. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1994; 75:127–132Google Scholar

27. Lenze EJ, Munin MC, Quear T, et al: The Pittsburgh Rehabilitation Participation Scale: reliability and validity of a clinician-rated measure of participation in acute rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85:380–384Google Scholar

28. Lenze EJ, Munin MC, Quear T, et al: Significance of poor patient participation in physical and occupational therapy for functional outcome and length of stay. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2004; 85:1599–1601Google Scholar

29. Marin RS, Fogel BS, Hawkins J, et al: Apathy: a treatable syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995; 7:23–30Google Scholar

30. Barbisoni P, Bertozzi B, Franzoni S, et al: Mood improvement in elderly women after in-hospital physical rehabilitation. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 1996; 77:346–349Google Scholar

31. Schubert DSP, Burns R, Paras W, et al: Decrease of depression during stroke and amputation rehabilitation. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 1992; 14:135–141Google Scholar

32. Politis AM, Vozzella S, Mayer LS, et al: A randomized, controlled, clinical trial of activity therapy for apathy in patients with dementia residing in long-term care. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2004; 19:1087–1094Google Scholar

33. Schulz R, Heckhausen J, O’Brien AT: Control and the disablement process in the elderly. J Soc Behav Personality 1994; 9:139–152Google Scholar

34. Feil D, Razani J, Boone K, et al: Apathy and cognitive performance in older adults adults with depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003; 18:479–485Google Scholar

35. Marin RS, Butters MA, Mulsant BH, et al: Apathy and executive function in depressed elderly. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2003; 16:112–116Google Scholar