The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticlesFull Access

The Parkinsonian Personality and Concomitant Depression

Published Online:

Abstract

Parkinson's disease (PD) has been associated with a distinctive parkinsonian personality, characterized by conscientiousness, punctuality, industriousness, and reduced novelty-seeking, as compared with healthy elderly persons. Similar traits are identified in relation to depression. The objective of the study was to elucidate the relationship between the parkinsonian personality and depression. Thirty-two depressed and 86 nondepressed PD patients and 30 healthy control subjects completed the NEO-Personality Inventory Revised Short Version. PD patients with depression displayed a distinct personality profile, with increased Neuroticism and reduced Extroversion, as compared with nondepressed PD patients and control subjects. It seems plausible that a subgroup of PD patients possesses a distinct personality profile that renders them sensitive to development of depression, although the reverse might also be possible.

Previous research suggests that Parkinson's disease (PD) is associated with particular personality traits, the so-called parkinsonian personality. Patients with PD have been described as unassuming, loyal, cautious, deferential to authority, rigid, and less novelty-seeking than healthy elderly persons.1,2 These traits may even be present premorbidly because of early degeneration of dopamine networks, and the traits then persist or increase in severity after disease onset.2,3 However, the existence of a unique parkinsonian personality is still controversial, and others have found no significant differences in personality profiles between PD and medical control subjects46 or twin control subjects discordant for PD.7

A possible explanation for the different results on the parkinsonian personality could be depression acting as a confounding factor. The relationship between concomitant depression and the parkinsonian personality has not been fully explored, but recent research suggests that PD with depression is associated with increased Harm Avoidance on the Cloningers Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire.8,9 Hence, Jacobs et al.10 suggested that the difference in Harm Avoidance between PD and control subjects could be explained by the 56% depression prevalence rate in their PD group. Similarly, Fujii et al.11 stated that the increased Harm Avoidance and decreased Novelty-Seeking in their PD group could be related to concomitant depression (as indicated by the use of antidepressants).

Hubble et al.3 found associations between premorbid introvert personality traits, and depression. This is in line with research on depression without PD, where associations between neuroticism and onset of depression in late life have been reported.12 Given the high depression rates (approximately 40%) in PD, there is a strong risk that reported personality changes are associated with depression.13

To our knowledge, only Menza and Mark14 have directly compared the personality profiles of PD patients with and without concomitant depression. They found the depressed group to score significantly higher on Harm Avoidance but found no differences between the two PD groups on Novelty-Seeking. However, it was not reported whether the personality profiles of the depressed and non-depressed PD groups differed from the control group.

The present study was designed to further elucidate the relationship between concomitant depression in PD and personality by directly comparing the personality profiles of depressed PD patients with healthy-control participants and PD patients without depression.

METHOD

Participants

A total of 480 outpatients with PD, residing in the region formerly known as Aarhus County, Denmark, were identified from a review of medical records, at the University hospitals of Aarhus. Some patients were identified by neurologists practicing in the county. PD was diagnosed in accordance with the United Kingdom Parkinson's Disease Brain Bank Diagnostic Criteria.15

A total of 272 patients were excluded for the following reasons: dementia (N=53), severe psychiatric illness other than depression (N=7), other neurological disorders than PD or head trauma with loss of consciousness (N=76), alcohol or substance abuse (N=5), moved outside the county or had Danish as a secondary language (N=15), had deep brain-stimulation implant (N=20), were under age 50 at disease onset (N=60), suspicion of other diagnosis than idiopathic PD (N=36).

The remaining 208 eligible patients were offered participation in the questionnaire study; 129 patients returned the questionnaires; 49 patients did not wish to participate; and 30 patients did not respond. The overall response rate was 62%. A further 11 patients were excluded from the study because of incomplete questionnaires. A total of 44% of the participants were men. None were taking antipsychotic medication. A total of 22 patients were being treated with antidepressants; 5 receiving tricyclic antidepressants, and the remaining 17 selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Seventy of the patients also participated in a still-unpublished neuropsychological study within 5 months of the questionnaire date (mean: 1.24 months; standard deviation [SD]: 2.17 months), where all had a Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) score above 24 (see Table 1). Of the remaining 48 patients, 15 had MMSE scores recorded in the medical records, and all were seen by a neurologist within 13 months of the questionnaire date (mean: 5.13 months; SD: 3.75 months). None was diagnosed with dementia or treated with antipsychotic medication at this time.

TABLE 1. Group Characteristics and Independent t-Test Comparisons and Chi-Square Values With Contingency Correction of Disease Variables
TABLE 1. Group Characteristics and Independent t-Test Comparisons and Chi-Square Values With Contingency Correction of Disease Variables
Enlarge table

Thirty healthy-control participants (13 men) were recruited by advertisements. They were all community residents without any neurological symptoms, severe chronic physical disease, or psychiatric illness. None had substance or alcohol abuse, or were taking medications with effects on the CNS. None received domiciliary care. All control participants also took part in a still-unpublished neuropsychological study and had an MMSE16 score ≥27 (mean: 29.10; SD: 0.22).

Procedure

The study was reported to the Danish Data Protection Agency and was carried out in accordance to the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants received a letter explaining the purpose of the study, a consent form, the questionnaires, and a prepaid envelope by mail. Nonrespondents received one reminder after 2 weeks. All participants were asked to fill out the questionnaires themselves and to double-check all answers before returning them. Hence, caregivers were not allowed to fill out the questionnaires on behalf of the patient.

Measures

Demographics and Health

Details on age at disease onset, general health, motor symptoms, medications, Hoehn and Yahr stage,17 MMSE (when available), and disease duration were recorded from medical records. Further information, on motor symptoms, motor fluctuations, and other symptoms were obtained from the participants via a questionnaire consisting of 1) a general health section (How would you describe your health? Do you take any medication for other diseases or ailments than PD? If yes, which? Do you receive domiciliary care?); 2) a PD symptom section (When did you first experience symptoms of PD [year]? When was the diagnosis made by a neurologist [year]? Where did the symptoms first appear [right arm, right leg, left arm, left leg]? Where are the dominant symptoms now [right arm, right leg, left arm, left leg]? Which of the following symptoms have you experienced within the last 30 days [I experience tremor or shakiness while resting; for instance, sitting with my hands in my lap? I experience tremor or shakiness while writing or doing other movements? Sometimes I “freeze” and have difficulties moving? I have difficulties keeping balance and tend to fall? I have noticed that my movements have become slower? I feel stiffness or rigidity in my muscles? I have involuntary big movements [dyskinesias] that are not tremor? Sometimes my medication stops working and I have trouble moving?); 3) a PD medication section (I would describe the effect of my medication as: good, moderate, limited, or poor? It is predictable when the effect of my medication wears off: yes, no, or it wears off in seconds? Have you experienced any of the following side effects in the last 30 days: nausea, vomiting, or loss of desire to eat? Sleeplessness, tiredness or other sleep disorders?).

The Short Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS–15)18

Severity of depressive symptoms was assessed with the Danish version of the GDS–15.19 The GDS–15 is a self-administered, 15-item questionnaire especially suitable for PD patients because it contains no somatic items.20 A cutoff score of 5 indicates clinically important depressive symptoms and has a sensitivity of 91% and a specificity of 72% when compared with validated psychiatric diagnosis.21

The NEO Personality Inventory, Short Version22

The Danish version of the NEO-Five Factor Inventory was used to assess five personality dimensions as derived from the five-factor model of personality. The five personality dimensions assessed are Neuroticism (tendency toward experiencing psychological distress or negative affectivity), Extroversion (tendency to be sociable, novelty-seeking, and adventurous), Openness (openness to internal and external stimuli, including imagination), Agreeableness (trustfulness, altruism, modesty), and Conscientiousness (self-discipline and competency). Importantly, the questionnaire assesses traits (stable characteristics of personality) as opposed to states (temporary psychological states and moods).

The questionnaire consists of 60 statements that the respondents rate on a 5-point Likert scale from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” The NEO-PI–R Short Version is a shortened version of the 240 item NEO-PI–R. However, correlations of 0.89–0.93 between the short and long version of the questionnaire have been reported.23 Validity and reliability of 0.73–0.84 have been reported for this scale.22

Statistical Methods

Data were analyzed with SPSS 17.00. The patient group was subdivided into a Depressed (PDd; with GDS score ≥5; N=32) and a Nondepressed (PD; GDS ≤4; N=86) group; t-test and chi-squared test were used to compare the two groups on clinical and demographic variables. One-way, between-group analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the PD, PDd, and control groups on demographic variables. Multiple analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was applied to compare the personality profiles of the PD, PDd, and control groups, with age and years of education as covariates. Also, a MANCOVA was performed using disease features as covariates. Preliminary assessments were performed to ensure that assumptions of normality, homogeneity of variance, linearity, and homogeneity of regression slopes were not violated. Whenever a significant main effect was found, group differences were tested by one-way, between-groups ANOVA, with Tukey's honestly significant difference [HSD] post-hoc comparison procedures.

To ensure the representativeness of the control-group, scores on the NEO-FFI were initially compared with the existing Danish normative material supplied with the questionnaire, and the scores were deemed comparable.24

RESULTS

The PD and control group did not differ significantly in terms of age (PD mean: 71.29 (7.45) years, and control mean: 69 (5.73) years; t[54.36]=1.840; p=0.071) or in years of higher education after municipal primary and lower secondary school (PD mean: 3.81 (2.81) years, and control mean: 4.37 (2.77) years; t [157] = –0.973; p=0.332).

Clinical and Demographic Variables

A chi-square test with contingency correction showed no significant gender difference between the PD (55 men) and PDd group (31 men); (χ2[1]; N=118)=0.347; p=0.556; Φ = –0.074). Group characteristics and comparisons of disease variables are shown in Table 1. The PDd group was significantly older at disease onset, had greater disease severity, and received significantly larger L-dopa doses than the PD group. The PD group received significantly more dopamine-agonist therapy than the PDd group. There were no significant differences in disease duration, MMSE score, use of MAO-B inhibitors, COMT inhibitors, or antidepressants between the two PD groups.

The symptomatology of the two PD patient groups was further compared, using a chi-square test for independence (with Yates Contingency correction). As shown in Table 2, the PDd group experienced significantly more action tremor, postural instability, and wearing-off episodes than the PD group. There was no significant difference between the two PD groups on resting tremor, bradykinesia, rigidity, freezing, or dyskinesia.

TABLE 2. Comparison of Parkinsonian Symptomatology, Using Chi-Square With Contingency Correction
TABLE 2. Comparison of Parkinsonian Symptomatology, Using Chi-Square With Contingency Correction
Enlarge table

The PD and PDd groups were compared with the control group on age and education by use of one-way ANOVA. Significant age differences were detected between the three groups (F[2, 145]=6.51; p=0.002). Post-hoc Tukey's HSD comparisons showed that the mean age for the PDd group (74.43 [7.54] years) was significantly higher than for the PD group (69.61 [7.10]) and the control group (69.02 [5.73]). The control and PD group did not differ significantly with regard to age. There was no significant difference in years of education among the three groups (F[2, 145]=0.491; p=0.613).

Personality Measures

MANCOVA was applied to assess differences in personality dimensions among the PD, PDd, and control groups. Because demographic characteristics of age and education have been found to affect personality, all analyses were performed with age and years of education as covariates. As shown in Table 3, there were no significant differences between the three groups on Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, or Openness. The PDd group was significantly lower on Extroversion and scored significantly higher on Neuroticism than the PD and control groups. There were no significant differences on personality traits between the control group and the nondepressed PD group.

TABLE 3. Group Comparisons of Personality Dimensions, MANCOVA
TABLE 3. Group Comparisons of Personality Dimensions, MANCOVA
Enlarge table

In order to investigate whether differences in disease features between PD and PDd influenced the differences in personality dimensions, we performed a MANCOVA, with disease severity, L-dopa use, and age at disease onset as covariates. The PDd group scored significantly higher on Neuroticism (F[1, 113]=14.644; p=0.000) and significantly lower on Extroversion (F[1, 113]=8.439; p=0.000), as compared with the PD group; there were no significant differences on Openness (F[1, 113]=2.450; p=0.120), Agreeableness (F[1, 113]=0.132; p=0.717), or Conscientiousness (F[1, 113]=1.125; p=0.291).

DISCUSSION

The purpose of this study was to explore the parkinsonian personality and its relation to concomitant depression. We found no evidence of a specific personality profile in nondepressed PD patients. However, depressed PD patients displayed traits similar to the personality profiles reported in other studies, with reduced Extroversion and increased Neuroticism. The depressed PD patients were significantly older than the PD patients without depression and the control participants. Differences in personality profiles did, however, remain significant after controlling for age. Also, the nondepressed PD group received significantly larger dopamine-agonist doses, which has been associated with a slight antidepressant effect. Hence, it is possible that depressive symptomatology would have been reduced in this group as a result of this medical regimen. Furthermore, the PDd group experienced significantly more action tremor, wearing-off, and postural instability. It is possible that more pronounced motor symptoms in this group could explain the lower Extroversion scores simply because of limitations imposed by the disease on physical mobility and social interactions. However, the results remained significant after controlling for disease severity. The increased tendency to wearing-off episodes in the PDd group may also be associated with an elevated propensity to experience fluctuations in affect, which could have an impact on the prevalence of depression in this group.

The study design does not allow for causal inferences between personality traits and depression in PD. Hence, it remains plausible that depression leads to changes in personality and vice versa. Furthermore, the co-occurrence of depression and high Neuroticism scores might be a reflection of the intertwined nature of traits (Neuroticism) and states (depression). In this regard, it may pose a particular challenge to distinguish Neuroticism from psychological state, since it displays some degree of correlation with several psychiatric symptoms,2527 and it has been suggested that a joint genetic factor underlies neuroticism and the risk of major depression.28 Along this line, studies of healthy elderly subjects without a history of depression have identified neuroticism as an important predictor for late-life onset of depression.12,29 Therefore, it is possible that increased neuroticism is manifest in a subgroup of PD patients premorbidly as a result of degeneration of dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area before the onset of depression. This subgroup could, in turn, subsequently be more susceptible to developing depression. Further studies are needed to ascertain such a relationship.

Interestingly, the present study failed to demonstrate differences in personality profiles between healthy elderly and nondepressed PD patients. Hence, unlike previous research, this study did not find evidence of a distinct parkinsonian personality applicable to PD, per se. One possible explanation for this finding is our use of the NEO-PI–R Short Version, whereas most of the recent research has used the TPQ (Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire) as a measure of personality traits.8,9 The TPQ assesses three types of temperament: Novelty-Seeking, Reward-Dependence, and Harm-Avoidance. The traits are hypothesized to be expressions of particular neurotransmitters; that is, novelty-seeking is dependent on dopamine, harm-avoidance on serotonin, and reward-dependence on norepinephrine. However, disruptions of these three pathways are also believed to be implicated in PD depression.30 Several studies have reported PD patients to be less novelty-seeking31 and more harm-avoidant,11,32,33 whereas others have only reported increased harm-avoidance.10 Interestingly, harm-avoidance is positively correlated with the severity of depression in PD as well as in non-parkinsonian subjects.33,34 The Harm-Avoidance trait on the TPQ is also positively correlated with Neuroticism on the NEO-PI–R and negatively correlated with Extroversion.35 In the same regard, TPQ Novelty-Seeking is positively correlated with NEO-PI–R Extroversion.35 Hence, it is interesting that studies using the TPQ found patients to differ on traits that were similar to the ones found in the present study of depressed PD patients. In relation to this research, the TPQ may face the same challenge of untangling contributions of PD and depression on personality profiles, because harm-avoidance appears to be especially sensitive to concomitant depression.36,37 Furthermore, even though novelty-seeking is generally not found to correlate with depression in PD, direct comparison of depressed and nondepressed PD patients may yield different results. This was in part addressed by Menza and Mark,14 who compared depressed and nondepressed PD patients by use of the TPQ. The authors reported a significant group difference on Harm-Avoidance only, and not on Novelty-Seeking. However, their patient group was subdivided into a depressed and a nondepressed group on the basis of the Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (using a cutoff score of 48, being 1 SD from the mean), which has since been criticized for containing too many somatic items that overlap with PD symptomatology, thereby risking inflated depression prevalence.38 Furthermore, no cutoff score for PD patients has been established on this scale, but, in medical settings, a cutoff score of ≥60 is recommended39 this is higher than the cutoff score adopted by Menza and Mark.14 Hence, there is a risk that depression rates were overestimated, which could have counterbalanced any potential difference in novelty-seeking between the two groups. Furthermore, the two PD groups were not compared with a control group. Further studies comparing depressed and nondepressed PD patients on the TPQ, using a standardized and validated measure of depression in PD, would be warranted.

Some limitations of the current study must be stressed. The study did not address the presence of apathy, which exists independently of depression in an estimated 12%–28.8% of PD patients.40,41 However, applying the TPQ, Pluck and Brown42 found no significant differences in personality measures between PD patients with high and low apathy scores. Unfortunately, the impact of apathy on NEO-PI–R has, to our knowledge, only been addressed in a study of college students, where students with high apathy scores were less agreeable, more introverted, and less conscientious.43 If such a constellation of personality traits were applicable to a sizable nondepressed apathetic subgroup in our study, we would have expected to find significant differences in these traits between the PD and control group. This however, was not the case. On the other hand, the existence of apathy co-occurring with depression in the PDd group may have magnified our findings, as, for instance, reduced Extroversion scores could have been due to apathy. Second, the study did not use measures of cognitive deficits or dementia other than the MMSE, which was not available for all participants. Hence, it remains possible that some patients, for whom MMSE scores were unavailable, had dementia when they participated in the study. However, the medical charts of the patients in question were reviewed again, and none were diagnosed with dementia. Lastly, to our knowledge, none of the personality questionnaires (the NEO-PI–R, TPQ, or Karolinska Scales of Personality) commonly used to assess personality in PD has been validated specifically in this population. Such validation would be very important to estimate whether the content of the personality measures are related to functional, cognitive, or behavioral disability caused by PD.

CONCLUSION

The personality profiles of depressed and nondepressed PD patients were compared with healthy elderly subjects. Increased Neuroticism and reduced Extroversion scores were detected only in depressed PD patients, but not in nondepressed patients. Although any causal link between depression and personality profiles cannot be established in the present study, it seems plausible that a subgroup of PD patients possess a distinct personality profile due to early monoaminergic and cholinergic degeneration. This could render them vulnerable to development of depression, although the reverse might also be possible.

The present findings need to be replicated and possibly elaborated to encompass the TPQ measure. Furthermore, we would recommend a validation of personality assessment questionnaires in PD populations.

Drs. Damholdt and Larsen are affiliated with the Department of Psychology, Aarhus University, in Aarhus C, Denmark; Dr. Østergaard is affiliated with the Department of Neurology at Aarhus University Hospital in Aarhus N, Denmark; Dr. Borghammer is affiliated with the PET Centre at Aarhus University Hospital.
Address correspondence to Malene Flensborg Damholdt, M.Sc., Ph.D., Fellow, Department of Psychology, Aarhus University, Jens Chr. Skous Vej 4, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark; (e-mail).

This study was supported by ”Civil Engineer Hans Lønborg Madsens Memorial Foundation10313120” and “The Hede Nielsen Family Foundation” to MFD, and The Danish Medical Research Council, The Lundbeck Foundation, and The Danish Parkinson Disease Association, to KØ.

None of the authors receive commercial or financial support that might present a conflict of interest in connection to the submitted article.

1. Eatough VM , Kempster PA , Stern GM, et al.: Premorbid personality and idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Adv Neurol 1990; 53:335–337MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Menza MA , Forman NE , Goldstein HS, et al.: Parkinson's disease, personality, and dopamine. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1990; 2:282–287LinkGoogle Scholar

3. Hubble JP , Venkatesh R , Hassanein RES, et al.: Personality and depression in Parkinson's disease. J Nerv Ment Dis 1993; 181:657–671Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Leiva-Santana C , Monge-Argiles JA , Galvan-Berenguer B: A study of personality in Parkinson's disease: the influence of motor and non-motor factors. [Estudio de la personalidad en la enfermedad de Parkinson: influencia de factores motores y no motores] Revista De Neurologia 2007; 45:7–12Google Scholar

5. Glosser G , Clark C , Freundlich B, et al.: A controlled investigation of current and premorbid personality: characteristics of Parkinson's disease patients. Mov Disord 1995; 10:201–206Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Poewe W , Karamat E , Kemmler GW, et al.: The premorbid personality of patients with Parkinson's disease: a comparative study with healthy controls and patients with essential tremor. Adv Neurol 1990; 53:339–342MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Heberlein I , Ludin H , Scholz J, et al.: Personality, depression, and premorbid lifestyle in twin pairs discordant for Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998; 64:262–266Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Cloninger CR: A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants: a proposal. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 44:573–588Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Cloninger CR , Przybeck TR , Svrakic DM: The Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychol Rep 1991; 69:1047–1057Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Jacobs H , Heberlein I , Vieregge A, et al.: Personality traits in young patients with Parkinson's disease. Acta Neurol Scand 2001; 103:82–87Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Fujii C , Harada S , Ohkoshi N, et al.: Cross-cultural traits for personality of patients with Parkinson's disease in Japan. Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatric Genetics 2000; 96:1–3Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Steunenberg B , Beekman ATF , Deeg DJH, et al.: Personality and the onset of depression in late life. J Affect Disord 2006; 92:243–251Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Starkstein SE , Merello M: Anxiety, phobias, apathy, and premorbid personality in Parkinson's disease, in Psychiatric and Cognitive Disorders in Parkinson's Disease. Edited by Starkstein SEMerello M. New York, Cambridge University Press; 2002, pp 114–129CrossrefGoogle Scholar

14. Menza MA , Mark MH: Parkinson's disease and depression: the relationship to disability and personality. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:165–169LinkGoogle Scholar

15. Hughes AJ , Daniel SE , Kilford L, et al.: Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992; 55:181–184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Folstein MF , Folstein SE , McHugh PR: Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Hoehn MM , Yahr MD: Parkinsonism: onset, progression, and mortality. Neurology 1967; 17:427–442Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Sheikh JI , Yesavage JA: Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin Gerontol 1986; 5:165–173CrossrefGoogle Scholar

19. Djernes JK , Kvist E , Olesen F, et al.: Validation of a Danish translation of the Geriatric Depression Scale–15 as a screening tool for depression among community-living, frail elderly. [Validering af Dansk oversættelse af Geriatric Depression Scale-15 som screeningsredskab for depression blandt hjemmeboende svage ældre] Ugeskrift for Laeger 2004; 166:905–909Google Scholar

20. Meara J , Mitchelmore E , Hobson P: Use of the GDS–15 Geriatric Depression Scale as a screening instrument for depressive symptomatology in patients with Parkinson's disease and their carers in the community. Age Ageing 1999; 28:35–38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. D'Ath P , Katona P , Mullan E, et al.: Screening, detection, and management of depression in elderly primary-care attenders, I: the acceptability and performance of the 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS–15) and the development of short versions. Fam Pract 1994; 11:260–266Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Costa PT , McCrae RR: Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychol Assess 1992; 4:5–13CrossrefGoogle Scholar

23. McCrae RR , John OP: An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. J Pers 1992; 60:175–215Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Costa PT , McCrae RR: NEO PI–R Manual Klinisk. Dansk Psykologisk Forlag 2006, 2:160Google Scholar

25. Miller TR: The psychotherapeutic utility of the five-factor model of personality: a clinician's experience. J Pers Assess 1991; Special Series: Clinical Use of the Five-Factor Model of Personality 1991; 57:415–433Google Scholar

26. Carey G , DiLalla DL: Personality and psychopathology: genetic perspectives. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:32–43Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Bienvenu OJ , Nestadt G , Samuels JF, et al.: Phobic, panic, and major depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:154–161Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Kendler KS , Kessler RC , Neale MC, et al.: The prediction of major depression in women: toward an integrated etiologic model. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1139–1148Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Duberstein PR , Pálsson SP , Waern M, et al.: Personality and risk for depression in a birth cohort of 70-year-olds followed for 15 years. Psychol Med 2008; 38:663–671Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30. Remy P , Doder M , Lees A, et al.: Depression in Parkinson's disease: loss of dopamine and noradrenaline innervations in the limbic system. Brain 2005; 128:1314–1322Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Menza MA , Golbe LI , Cody RA, et al.: Dopamine-related personality traits in Parkinson's disease. Neurology 1993; 43:505–508Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Kaasinen V , Nurmi E , Bergman J, et al.: Personality traits and brain dopaminergic function in Parkinson's disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001; 98:13272–13277Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Tomer R , Aharon-Peretz J: Novelty-seeking and harm-avoidance in Parkinson's disease: effects of asymmetric dopamine deficiency. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004; 75:972–975Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Hirano S , Sato T , Narita T, et al.: Evaluating the state dependency of the temperament and character inventory dimensions in patients with major depression: a methodological contribution. J Affect Disord 2002; 69:31–38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35. De Fruyt F , Van De Wiele L , Van Heeringen C: Cloninger's psychobiological model of temperament and character and the five-factor model of personality. Pers Indiv Diff 2000; 29:441–452CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36. Brown SL , Svrakic DM , Przybeck TR, et al.: The relationship of personality to mood and anxiety states: a dimensional approach. J Psychiatr Res 1992; 26:197–211Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Hansenne M , Reggers J , Pinto E, et al.: Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) and depression. J Psychiatr Res 1999; 33:31–36Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Schrag A , Barone P , Brown RG, et al.: Depression rating scales in Parkinson's disease: critique and recommendations. Mov Disord 2007; 22:1077–1092Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Okimoto JT , Barnes RF , Veith R: Screening for depression in geriatric medical patients. Am J Psychiatry 1982; 139:799–802Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40. Starkstein SE , Mayberg HS , Preziosi TJ, et al.: Reliability, validity, and clinical correlates of apathy in Parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1992; 4:134–139LinkGoogle Scholar

41. Kirsch-Darrow L , Fernandez HF , Marsiske M, et al.: Dissociating apathy and depression in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006; 67:33–38Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42. Pluck GC , Brown RG: Apathy in Parkinson's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 73:636–642Google Scholar

43. Bjornsen CA , Scepansky JA , Suzuki A: Apathy and personality traits among college students: a cross-cultural comparison. Coll Stud J 2007; 41Google Scholar