The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Decreased treatment adherence in patients with diabetes mellitus type 1 (type 1 DM) may reflect impairments in decision-making and underlying associated deficits in working memory and executive functioning. Other factors, including comorbid major depression, may also interfere with decision-making. The authors sought to review the clinically relevant characteristics of decision-making in type 1 DM by surveying the literature on decision-making by patients with type 1 DM. Deficiencies in decision-making in patients with type 1 DM or their caregivers contribute to treatment nonadherence and poorer metabolic control. Animal models of type 1 DM reveal deficits in hippocampal-dependent memory tasks, which are reversible with insulin. Neurocognitive studies of patients with type 1 DM reveal lowered performance on ability to apply knowledge to solve problems in a new situation and acquired scholarly knowledge, psychomotor efficiency, cognitive flexibility, visual perception, speed of information-processing, and sustained attention. Other factors that might contribute to poor decision-making in patients with type 1 DM, include “hypoglycemia unawareness” and comorbid major depression (given its increased prevalence in type 1 DM). Future studies utilizing novel treatment strategies to help patients with type 1 DM make better decisions about their disease may improve their glycemic control and quality of life, while minimizing the impact of end-organ disease.

Much of the burden of managing patients with diabetes mellitus type 1 (type 1 DM) rests with the patient and/or family members.1 Decreased adherence to prescribed treatment of type 1 DM is a major challenge,2 as such treatment nonadherence or “mismanagement” leads to increased healthcare costs, medical complications, hospitalizations, and fatal outcomes (such as diabetic ketoacidosis).3 Given that poor decision-making may contribute to treatment nonadherence in this patient population, we reviewed previous studies scrutinizing the decision-making of patients with type 1 DM and examined how various factors, such as neurocognitive deficits, “hypoglycemia unawareness,” and comorbid depression, might also contribute to impaired decision-making.

We conducted multiple MEDLINE searches for 2000–2011 using terms such as “diabetes,” “type 1 diabetes,” “decision-making,” “problem-solving,” “cognition,” “depression,” and “pathophysiology.” We supplemented the online searches with manual reviews of article reference lists and selected articles for further review by author consensus.

What Types of Decisions Do Patients With Type 1 DM Have to Make?

Decision-making encompasses a complex set of processes that requires various higher-order cognitive functions through which individuals regulate their actions, thoughts, and emotions according to goals, current physiological or psychological states, and environmental conditions.4 Patients with type 1 DM face a relentlessly changing environment that requires executive functioning and working memory. Executive functioning consists of an array of cognitive functions, including attention, problem-solving, verbal reasoning, inhibition, mental flexibility, multi-tasking, initiation, and monitoring of actions.5 Working memory, another of the “executive functions” is the ability of an individual to actively hold and manipulate information.6 Related concepts are those of health literacy (e.g., the ability to complete basic reading required to treat their illness),7 and health numeracy, defined as the skills needed to comprehend and use quantitative health information such as rudimentary mathematical skills (e.g., to calculate amounts of insulin and judge carbohydrate content of foods),8 the ability to use non-text formats, such as graphs, and the capacity for oral communication.9

In persons with type 1 DM, salient decisions in diabetes management revolve around pre-meal insulin administration, and require an extensive collection of cognitive skills. Strategic dosing of pre-meal insulin depends on several factors (e.g., visual processing of blood glucose reading, estimation of anticipated meal size, accurate recollection of antecedent activity level, memory about experiences under similar conditions).10 The timing of pre-meal insulin injections or boluses is related to the level of a person’s pre-meal glycemia. When blood glucose levels are above a patient’s desired target, increasing the time between insulin administration and meal consumption will enable the insulin to take effect. Conversely, if pre-meal blood glucose levels are under a patient’s target range, administration of insulin should be delayed until just before eating.10 Timing of pre-meal insulin injections is of great consequence for parents of young children with type 1 DM, as they must balance the benefit of pre-meal insulin administration with the possibility that the child may decide not to eat, and thereby suffer a hypoglycemic episode.

Neurocognitive Deficits in Executive Functioning and Problem-Solving Skills That Lead to Poor Glycemic Control

Proper neurotransmitter function is critically important for decision-making; unpredictable levels of glucose (both high and low) in patients with poorly-controlled diabetes may deteriorate neurotransmitter functioning and contribute to cognitive dysfunction.11 Of note is the idea that decisions to be made in situations of uncertainty (which require risk-taking and exploratory choices) involve norepinephrine in the orbitofrontal cortex, whereas predictable environments may lead to consideration of longer-delayed rewards, dependent upon serotonin in the dorsal prefrontal cortex and dorsal striatum.12 Also, expectation of a high reward may trigger an individual’s choosing an action disregarding a large risk, a decision that is influenced, in part, by dopamine in the anterior cingulate cortex.12 Laboratory studies of animals indicate that not only turnover rate, but also steady-state level of monoamines may be altered in the brains of animals with diabetes.13 Alloxan (45 mg/kg) diabetic rats untreated for 30 days and showing hyperglycemia (>250 mg%) had significant increases of 5-HT level in the striatum, midbrain, pons medulla, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex and elevated 5-HIAA levels in the striatum, hippocampus, and midbrain. Enhancement of biosynthesis and/or decline in metabolism rates could explain these rises in levels of 5-HT and 5-HIAA.13

Experiments performed with streptozotocin-induced type 1 DM rats 30 days after injection (when diabetes was well stabilized and streptozotocin was washed away) showed biochemical changes in central and peripheral catecholaminergic systems.14 Dopamine content was reduced in the nigrostriatal system (both in the midbrain, where neuronal bodies are present, and in the synaptic terminals of the striatum). Alterations of norepinephrine occurred in the sympathetic nervous system (but not the CNS), with increases in the cardiac ventricles and decreases in the stellate ganglia and the blood serum; reduced norepinephrine synthesis and increased storage due to a reduced release from synaptic vesicles may explain these findings.14

Exogenously-administered insulin may lead to a reduction of monoamine turnover during hyperglycemia and an increase during hypoglycemia.15 Furthermore, in the setting of hyperglycemia and ischemia, the accretion of the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate can cause CNS damage.16

In children with type 1 DM, the patient and his or her family must organize several critical daily management tasks (including dietary intake, exercise, blood glucose monitoring, and insulin administration).5 In one study, 235 children with type 1 DM (mean age: 10.5 [SD: 1] years; 108 boys and 127 girls) and their primary caregivers were separately administered the Diabetes Self-Management Profile to assess treatment adherence.5 The executive functioning of the children was measured with the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning, and their glycemic control was assessed by HbA1c levels. The researchers found that executive functioning (e.g., planning, problem-solving, organization, working memory) was associated with adherence, which, in turn, was related to diabetes control. In fact, structural-equation modeling showed that adherence mediated the relationship between executive functioning and glycemic control.5

Problem-solving is an element of executive functioning that involves analysis of the problem, generation of possible solutions, evaluation of the risk/benefit profile of those solutions, and outcome analysis.17 Cross-sectional studies of adults with type 1 DM consistently demonstrate that ineffective/poor problem-solving ability is associated with poorer glycemic control.18 A recent study showed that children and adolescents with type 1 DM whose caregivers lack sufficient skill for responding to, and managing, blood glucose fluctuations may be at special risk for poor diabetes-related outcomes.17 Diabetes Problem Solving Interview (DPSI) data and measures of diabetes management were obtained at baseline from 114 youths (ages 9–14.5) and 109 caregivers. Glycosylated hemoglobin (HbA1c) was measured quarterly over 9 months. For caregivers, but not youths, low DPSI scores (indicating poor problem-solving skills) were significantly associated with worse HbA1c over 9 months.17

Decision-making competence is the ability or capacity to form flexible and effective plans for managing different situations in the midst of pursuing personal goals.(19,20) One study21 examined decision-making competence in a sample of adolescents (34 boys and 29 girls) between the ages of 11 and 17 years (mean age: 13.3 [SD: 1.77) years, with type 1 DM, using the Melbourne Decision-Making Questionnaire (MDMQ).22 The MDMQ has 22 items that measure both competent (e.g., “I like to consider all the alternatives.”) and maladaptive decision-making (hypervigilant, buck-passing, and procrastination). Hypervigilant decision-making is characterized by the frantic search for a way out of dilemmas and impulsively choosing hastily contrived solutions that seem to promise immediate relief.22 Emotional excitement, perseveration, and limited attention cause the hypervigilant decision-maker to overlook the full range of consequences of choice. Higher levels of adolescent hypervigilance were associated with lower parent report of adherence (r = −0.33; p <0.01).21 The authors noted that the questionnaires such as the MDMQ may not reflect the processes involved in diabetes-related decisions, as diabetes-related decisions must be made on a daily basis, carry additional emotional significance, and may have short- and long-term health consequences. Relevant in this regard is that adolescents may possess competent decision-making skills in peer relationships and academics, but not about type 1 DM care, possibly explaining why this study did not demonstrate an association between adolescent decision-making competence and HbA1c values. Indeed, the treatment adherence of the parents was not measured, thereby potentially counteracting the detrimental effect of adolescent hypervigilance upon glucose control.

Neurocognitive Deficits and Pathophysiologic Changes in the Brain: Substrates for Impaired Decision-Making in Patients With Type 1 DM

Cognitive impairment may also contribute to impaired decision-making in patients with type 1 DM. Mice with streptozotocin-induced diabetes exhibit significant memory retention deficits on hippocampus-dependent, active avoidance tasks, and deficits in performance on spatial learning and memory tasks that worsened with task complexity.23 Hyperglycemia also appears to exert a deleterious effect on the hippocampal neurons (and their neuroplasticity) of rats, causing decreased long-term potentiation (i.e., decreased sprouting), which is related to the degree of hyperglycemia.24 Fortunately, sprouting improved with glycemic control.25 Moreover, in animal models, administration of exogenous insulin improves performance on some learning tasks and corrects the learning and memory deficits associated with streptozotocin-induced diabetes. That is, insulin treatment reverses deficits in hippocampus-dependent, active avoidance tasks in streptozotocin-treated diabetic rats,26 and prevents streptozotocin-induced impairments in water-maze learning (and hippocampal long-term potentiation).27

Whether peripheral insulin is able to traverse the blood–brain barrier to act upon CNS insulin receptors remains an unresolved and controversial issue. Insulin receptors are densely expressed in the hippocampus, and brain insulin receptors may simply be activated by intrinsic CNS or insulin-related molecules.28 Thus, these preclinical models of type 1 DM have revealed cognitive deficits in memory that likely represent in humans a specific abnormality in hippocampus-dependent declarative memory,29 and insulin is thought to act at this brain structure to facilitate learning and memory.30

In a metaanalysis of 33 studies on cognitive performance in patients with type 1 DM, Brands and coworkers documented a significantly lowered performance on ability to apply knowledge to solve problems in a new situation and also on acquired scholarly knowledge (d = −0.7; moderate effect size), psychomotor efficiency (d = −0.6), cognitive flexibility (d = −0.5), visual perception (d = −0.4), speed of information-processing (d = −0.3, mild effect size), and sustained attention (d = −0.3).31 Perhaps not surprisingly, lowered cognitive performance was associated with presence of microvascular complications (e.g., retinopathy, nephropathy).

Interestingly, the cross-sectional studies (3238) included in the metaanalysis revealed no consistent relationship between disease duration and cognition, perhaps, at least in part, because persons may differ in terms of the magnitude of their diabetes (dys)control.31 Nevertheless, as the duration of type 1 DM increases, development of diffuse brain degeneration, demyelination of cranial nerves and spinal cord, and nerve fibrosis and pseudocalcinosis within the CNS may ultimately impair cognition and decision-making.11 Imaging studies of brains of patients with type 1 DM have revealed white-matter microstructural deficits, cerebral atrophy, increases in regional cerebral blood flow, leukoaraiosis (periventricular white-matter disease), lower gray-matter density, and reductions in hippocampal and amygdalar volume.39 These structural changes may contribute to consequent impairment of decision-making.

One study used blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) functional magnetic resonance imaging during euglycemic (5.0 mmol/L) and hypoglycemic (2.8 mmol/L) hyperinsulinemic clamps to investigate the effects of acute hypoglycemia on working memory and brain functioning in patients with Type 1 DM.40 The experiment compared brain activation response to a working-memory task (WMT) in subjects with type 1 DM (N=16) to that of age-matched non-diabetic control subjects (N=16). BOLD activation was increased and deactivation was decreased in type 1 DM versus control subjects, which indicates that subjects with type 1 DM have reduced cerebral efficiency (i.e., they require a higher level of brain activation to achieve the same level of cognitive performance as control subjects).40 Thus, taken together, decreased treatment adherence of patients with type 1 DM may result from impairments in decision-making and underlying deficits in working memory and executive functioning.

Misinterpretation of Somatic Clues and “Hypoglycemia Unawareness” May Contribute to Poor Decision-Making in Type 1 Diabetes

Patients with type 1 DM often depend on “body listening” to pick up bodily cues indicative of hypo- or hyperglycemia.41 Atypical somatic cues of hypoglycemia and hyperglycemia for some patients may involve changes in the taste of saliva, mood alterations, changes in the taste of water, alterations in libido, changes in energy levels, dryness in the mouth, visual changes, or a change in the sensation of lips or tongue.42 In order to manipulate exogenous factors (such as insulin, dietary choices, and physical activity levels), patients with type 1 DM need to know what their blood glucose is and in which direction it is going.43 For instance, a blood glucose level of 60 mg/dl that is rising may need no intervention, whereas a level of 65 mg/dl that is failing may require immediate consumption of fast-acting carbohydrates. Although type 1 DM patients clearly became symptomatic during hypoglycemia, pattern of symptoms vary widely; and studies of adults (ages 18–44)44 and adolescents (ages 11–19)45 have shown that most patients are unable to accurately estimate their blood glucose level.44 Potential explanation for their misinterpretation is that patients monitor symptoms that do not accurately discriminate between hyperglycemia and hypoglycemia, or “feel” certain symptoms of a blood glucose fluctuation, but inaccurately interpret them.

Thus, the “somatic marker hypothesis,” as proposed by Bechara and colleagues,46 may be especially salient to conceptualize decision-making in patients with type 1 DM. This hypothesis holds that decision-making is a process guided by emotions and that overt reasoning is preceded by a nonconscious biasing step. This process employs neural systems (such as those regulating homeostasis, emotion, and feeling),46 other than those that support declarative knowledge.47 Anticipatory skin-conductance responses, an autonomic “somatic marker” of feelings generated by secondary emotions, may occur during decision-making tasks such as the Iowa Gambling Task.48 According to the “somatic marker hypothesis,” decision-making is a process influenced by marker signals (e.g., “gut feeling” or “hunch”) regarding the internal milieu, and a defect in emotion and feeling interferes with decision-making.49 Emotions rely on the limbic system, which includes brainstem reward-processing structures (e.g., ventral tegmental area), areas of the midbrain and cortex to which they project (e.g., nucleus accumbens and ventromedial frontal, orbitofrontal, and anterior cingulate cortex), and other areas such as the insular cortex and amygdala.50

In addition to misinterpretation of somatic cues, “hypoglycemia unawareness” may also contribute to poor decision-making in patients with type 1 DM. Episodes of hypoglycemia cause “hypoglycemia unawareness” (by lessening sympatho-adrenal and associated neurogenic symptoms to a certain level of subsequent hypoglycemia), defective glucose counter-regulation (via lowering epinephrine responses to a given level of subsequent hypoglycemia in the setting of absent decrements in insulin and absent increments in glucagon), and a lowered hypoglycemic threshold for hypoglycemic symptoms. (5154) These factors further increase the risk of severe hypoglycemia during insulin therapy of type 1 DM.55

Cerebral neuronal activation increases during symptomatic hypoglycemia, and, in “hypoglycemia unawareness,” this activation is reduced.56 Habituation (desensitization of physiologic response to stressor)57 to recurrent hypoglycemia involves differential involvement of cortical mechanisms involved in learning and conditioning (rather than, or in addition to, hypothalamic glucose-setting alterations).55 The pattern of decreased activation in stress pathways (e.g., amygdala and hypothalamus) and intact activation of brain regional networks subserving hedonic responses (e.g., motivation [ventral striatum] and reward-perception [lateral orbitofrontal cortex]) seen in the unaware subjects (as compared with aware subjects) suggests that the experience of hypoglycemia may be not just subjectively neutral but, instead, subjectively rewarding to the person. This phenomenon has implications for decision-making, as evidenced by a study showing that patients with hypoglycemia-unawareness were significantly less adherent to agreed-upon changes to insulin regimens designed to avoid hypoglycemia (hypoglycemia-awareness can be restored by hypoglycemia-avoidance) than their hypoglycemia-aware counterparts, despite increased clinical contact.58 These findings are compatible with habituation to hypoglycemic stress. Notably, this failure to perceive a situation (e.g., hypoglycemia) as dangerous undermines motivation, ability to change behavior, and, ultimately, treatment adherence.59 The authors suggest that behavioral strategies addressing habituation may help restore hypoglycemia-awareness and protect against severe hypoglycemia.

One study of cognitive functioning before, during, and after hypoglycemia in two groups of adult type 1 diabetic patients (ages 18–45), 20 with normal hypoglycemia awareness (NHA) and 16 with impaired hypoglycemia awareness (IHA) revealed that performance in the NHA group was impaired on cognitive tasks of the four-choice reaction-time test, Digit Symbol Substitution Task, and Trail-Making B test during hypoglycemia (and remained impaired for up to 75 minutes on the four-choice reaction-time test).60 Remarkably, cognitive performance did not deteriorate significantly during hypoglycemia in the IHA group (with the exception of the Digit Symbol Substitution Task after 60 minutes of hypoglycemia). Adaptation to low blood glucose had apparently occurred in the IHA patients, as shown by preservation of their cognitive functioning at a blood glucose level of 2.5 mmol/l. The IHA group had a longer duration of diabetes (median: 33.5 years [range: 22–43]) than the NHA group (29 [19–44] years; p <0.001). The IHA group also evidenced a higher prevalence of microvascular complications (six patients in the IHA group and one patient in the NHA group; χ2=5.994; p=0.013). Comparisons of gender, age, A1c, and BMI were nonsignificant. In this study, IHA was associated with “protection” against cognitive dysfunction during hypoglycemia.61 These individuals did not show the cognitive dysfunction of the NHA patients, which potentially serves as a warning signal (somatic marker) of impending danger, allowing NHA patients the opportunity to decide to take action to increase their blood glucose.

Fortunately, if patients can adhere to programs consisting of strict monitoring to avoid even mild hypoglycemia episodes, blunted counterregulatory hormonal responses and autonomic symptoms improve.62 Also, psychosocial interventions may help with these problems in body awareness. For instance, Blood Glucose Awareness Training (BGAT), an 8-week psychoeducational training program, has been shown to improve an individual’s ability to detect, anticipate, avoid, and treat extremes in blood glucose levels.43 More recently, 4 weeks of real-time continuous glucose monitoring (CGM) with preset alarms at specific glucose levels have been a useful tool to achieve avoidance of hypoglycemia in adolescents with type 1 DM with hypoglycemia unawareness, and it improved their counterregulatory response to hypoglycemia (as measured by epinephrine response during hyperinsulinemic hypoglycemic clamp studies) as compared with adolescents with type 1 DM with hypoglycemia-unawareness in the standard therapy group.63

Comorbid Depression May Contribute to Decision-Making Impairments in Patients With Type 1 DM

Patients with type 1 DM and comorbid depression may plausibly suffer from poor decision-making (Table 1). Accumulating evidence suggests that adults with type 1 DM are at least twice as likely as non-diabetic individuals to exhibit clinically significant depression.64 Indeed, the Coronary Artery Calcification in Type 1 Diabetes Study, which showed that the 458 participants with Type 1 DM (47% male, age 44 [SD: 9] years, Type 1 DM duration 29 [SD: 9] years) exhibit a prevalence rate of depression of 17.5%, versus the prevalence rate of 5.7% of 546 non-diabetic, age- and gender-matched control subjects (51% male, age 47 [SD: 9] years; p <0.0001),65 as assessed by Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)66 score >14. Also, more participants with type 1 DM were classified as depressed than those in the control group when the definition included current antidepressant use or BDI-II score >14 (32% versus 16%; p <0.0001). Furthermore, those type 1 DM participants with at least one diabetes complication (retinopathy, blindness, neuropathy, diabetes-related amputation, and kidney or pancreas transplantation; N=209) scored significantly higher on the BDI-II (mean BDI-II score: 10.7 [SD: 9.3]) than participants with type 1 DM without complications (mean BDI-II score: 6.4 [SD: 6.3]; p <0.0001). The cause–effect relationship could not be determined because of the cross-sectional nature of this study.65

TABLE 1. Comorbid Depression May Contribute to Impaired Decision-Making in Patients With Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus
Increased everyday decisional conflict and rumination, low self-efficacy, and lack of concentration may lead to poor adaptation to Type 1 DM and its variations over time.
Inward focus may impair ability to engage in collaborative/alliance work with family members and healthcare providers.
Limited ability to shift behavior under changing contingencies and altered reward-processing, which may involve an inflexible, generalized response to reward.
Impaired risk-related behavior can have potentially serious short- and long-term consequences.
Disruption of limbic-cortical pathways in depressive illness causes alteration of feeling and emotion, and consequently may lead to poor decision-making in patients with depressive illness. Comorbid depressive disorders may contribute to cognitive deficits in patients with Type 1 DM through alterations in these limbic-cortical networks.
TABLE 1. Comorbid Depression May Contribute to Impaired Decision-Making in Patients With Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus
Enlarge table

The ruminative, perseverant thoughts characteristic of depressive illness may impair the ability of patients with type 1 DM to balance everyday responsibilities with the prioritization of personal tasks critical to maintaining their physical health.67 A study of decision-making in depressed patients (without type 1 DM) used a one-item self-report measure to assess decisional conflict (“Do you feel conflicted when you have to make a decision?”) and a 20-item questionnaire for measuring ongoing processes during decision-making (Processes of a Decision-Making Questionnaire).67 The results showed that depressed patients experience increased levels of day-to-day decisional conflict (the aversive experience that accompanies indecisiveness), when compared with patients without depression. Furthermore, depressed patients often suffer from a lack of concentration, low self-efficacy, and rumination. Depressed individuals focus their attention inward and evaluate their competence, whereas healthy individuals focus upon the task. The inward focus of these depressed patients may hamper their collaboration with potentially helpful family members and accessing resources of healthcare providers.41

Decision-making is a higher-order cognitive function that involves the ability to choose between competing actions that are associated with varying levels of risk and reward.68 Reward-related decision-making was examined within a longitudinal study of 221 11-year-old boys, 25 of whom had a depressive disorder at age 10 or 11.69 Participants completed a behavioral decision-making task called the reward-contingent decision (RCD) paradigm 70,71 (see Table 2) which involves reward-related decisions under conditions of uncertainty and is considered to be emotional in nature. Boys with depression failed to distinguish between options involving small or large possible reward under conditions involving a high probability of winning. The results indicate that depression might involve a rigid, generalized strategy for responding to reward and limited ability to shift behavior under changing contingencies. Impaired flexible decision-making has also been demonstrated in adults with major depressive disorder (MDD) (N=19, mean age: 35.8 [SD: 10.1]) as compared with healthy controls (N=20; mean age: 35.1 [SD: 9.3])72 during the standard Iowa Gambling Test (IGT)73 and the contingency-shifting variant, IGT.(74,75) Analysis of the contingency-shift phase demonstrated that individuals with depression had difficulties determining when a previously bad contingency became favorable. Mental inflexibility in patients with type 1 DM and comorbid depression may impair their ability to vary strategies for changing conditions and thereby contribute to dangerous fluctuations in blood glucose.

TABLE 2. Frequent Paradigms Used to Study Decision-Making
ParadigmDescriptionNeuropsychological Target
Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)Selection of cards of four different stacks, with different monetary gains and penaltiesRisk and reward processing
Modified IGT (contingency-shift)A two-phase IGT in which the reward/risk characteristics of the decks are systematically changedSet-shifting as a measure of flexibility and adaptability
Cambridge Gambling Task79Probabilistic choice of finding a hidden token behind 10 boxesRisk-assessment without learning confounds
Wheel of FortuneTwo-choice task with probabilistic monetary outcomesSeparately targets reward and punishment processing
TABLE 2. Frequent Paradigms Used to Study Decision-Making
Enlarge table

One functional MRI study investigated how neural correlates of reward-related decision-making in young people (ages 9–17) with MDD differ from those in typically-developing control children.76 The reward task (adapted from Rogers et al.)71 involves choices about possible rewards involving varying magnitude and probability of reward; group differences in two reward processes, decision-making/anticipation and outcome, were examined. In the decision phase, those with depression exhibited a pattern consistent with decreased emotional reactivity to reward: blunted responses in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), bilateral caudate, and inferior orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) bilaterally, especially during high-magnitude reward conditions. The participants with depression also exhibited increased response in the middle and superior OFC bilaterally (especially to low-magnitude reward), a pattern consistent with over-regulation. Depression is also associated with generally diminished responses, particularly in conditions of small-magnitude reward, as evidenced by blunted response in the ACC, caudate, and OFC (particularly during loss and low-magnitude reward, but increased response in the amygdala). After high-magnitude rewards, those with depression exhibited a greater response than control participants in the amygdala bilaterally and the inferior OFC.76

In another neuroimaging study using fMRI,68 22 adolescents with no personal or family history of psychiatric illness and 22 adolescents with MDD were administered a two-choice decision-making task involving probabilistic monetary rewards with varying levels of risk: the Wheel of Fortune.77 During risky decision-making, healthy adolescents used the brain regions involved in inhibitory control (right lateral OFC), whereas depressed adolescents engaged areas involved in conflict monitoring (right caudal ACC). The authors hypothesized that reduced inhibitory control, as reflected by reduced activation of the right lateral OFC in depressed adolescents, may provide a neurobiological explanation for impulsivity (e.g., suicidal behavior and substance abuse among depressed youth).68

Because patients with type 1 DM exhibit an increased prevalence of depressive symptoms, and, as decision-making is impaired in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD), the “somatic marker hypothesis”46 may be especially relevant in conceptualizing decision-making in patients with type 1 DM and comorbid MDD. Mayberg has proposed a working model of depression, implicating failure of the coordinated interactions of a distributed network of limbic–cortical pathways.78 Disruption of limbic–cortical pathways in depressive illness causes alteration of feeling and emotion, and consequently may lead to poor-decision making in patients with depressive illness. Comorbid depressive disorders may contribute to cognitive deficits in patients with Type 1 DM through alterations in these limbic–cortical networks via perturbations in insulin-glucose homeostasis, adipokine synthesis and secretion, intracellular signaling cascades, mitochondrial respiration (reactive oxygen species), and immuno-inflammatory processes (proinflammatory cytokines, acute phase reactants, and cellular adhesion molecules).28

Despite the multiple areas in which decision-making is impaired in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD), no studies have utilized sophisticated, “in-the-laboratory” decision testing (Table 2) of patients with type 1 DM and comorbid depression to compare them to non-depressed patients with type 1 DM. The extent to which impaired decision-making contributes to the poorer outcome of patients with type 1 DM and comorbid depression remains to be determined.

Conclusions and Future Directions

A small extant literature documents that those patients with type 1 DM suffer neurocognitive deficits of executive functioning and working memory, which is associated with poorer glycemic control. Other contributors to poor decision-making in type 1 DM likely include neurocognitive deficits, “hypoglycemia unawareness,” and comorbid depression. The overlapping fields of neurocognitive psychology, endocrinology, and affective-disorders research can now sustain integrative research for screening of patients with type 1 DM for deficits in cognitive dysfunction and decision-making, discerning the impact of comorbid depressed mood, and whether treatment to improve decision-making in patients with type 1 DM will improve both glycemic and mood outcomes. Cross-sectional studies can be performed to measure the impact of brain glucose levels (e.g., with a glucose clamp) upon decision-making (e.g., using the Iowa Gambling task). Longitudinal studies will further clarify the impact of poor decision-making on diabetes outcomes, and, conversely, the contribution of duration and severity of type 1 DM upon one’s decision-making capacity. State-of-the-art functional neuroimaging can further reveal the underlying neuronal networks whereby hypoglycemia (or hyperglycemia) impairs decision-making in patients with type 1 DM, without and with comorbid depressed mood. This new knowledge, will help develop novel treatment strategies to help these patients make better decisions about their disease, thereby improving their glycemic control and quality of life, while minimizing the impact of end-organ disease.

From the Dept. of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL; the Diabetes Research Institute, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, and the Mailman Center for Child Development and Dept. of Pediatrics, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine.
Address correspondence to Dr. Nemeroff ().

James K. Rustad, M.D., has no financial disclosures to report.

Dominique L. Musselman, M.D., M.S., received grant support from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), The DANA Foundation, and Forest Laboratories, Inc.

Jay S. Skyler, M.D., reports being on the Board of Directors and owning stock in Amylin Pharmaceuticals, DexCom Inc., and Moerae Matrix; being on an Advisory Board for Sanofi; being a consultant for BD, Exsulin, and Gilead; and receiving grant support from Bayhill, Halozyme, Intuity, and Osiris.

Della Matheson, R.N., C.D.E., is on the Educational Advisory Board of DexCom and is a Certified Diabetes Pump Trainer for Animus Insulin Pump Co.

Alan Delamater, Ph.D., has no financial disclosures to report.

Norma S. Kenyon is a Scientific Advisor to the FDA, member of the Scientific Advisory Board of the Juvenile Diabetes Research Foundation International, consultant to the Diabetes Research Institute Foundation and a member of the advisory council, National Institute Allergy and Infectious Disease. She has stock options worth zero dollars in Converge Biotech, Inc.

Ricardo Cáceda, M.D., Ph.D., receives funding from NIMH, NIDA, NIAAA, the Arsht Foundation and is partial owner of MyHealthWin LLC.

Charles B. Nemeroff, M.D., Ph.D. discloses the following: Research/Grants: National Institutes of Health (NIH), Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ); Consulting: Xhale, Takeda, SK Pharma, Shire, Roche, Lilly; Stockholder: CeNeRx BioPharma, PharmaNeuroBoost, Revaax Pharma, Xhale, NovaDel Pharma; other financial interests: CeNeRx BioPharma, PharmaNeuroBoost; patents: method and devices for transdermal delivery of lithium (US 6,375,990B1); method of assessing antidepressant drug therapy via transport inhibition of monoamine neurotransmitters by ex vivo assay (US 7,148,027B2); Scientific Advisory Boards: American Foundation for Suicide Prevention (AFSP), CeNeRx BioPharma, National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD), Xhale, PharmaNeuroBoost, Anxiety Disorders Association of America (ADAA), Skyland Trail, AstraZeneca Pharmaceuticals (2009); Board of Directors: AFSP, Mt. Cook Pharma (2010), NovaDel (2011), Skyland Trail, Gratitude America, ADAA.

With the exception of the eight authors, there are no other contributors to this manuscript.

The authors thank Theodore A. Stern, M.D., for his helpful critique of the manuscript.

References

1 Thorne S, Paterson B, Russell C: The structure of everyday self-care decision making in chronic illness. Qual Health Res 2003; 13:1337–1352Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Cramer JA: A systematic review of adherence with medications for diabetes. Diabetes Care 2004; 27:1218–1224Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Morris AK, Boyle DIR: Adherence to insulin treatment, glycemic control, and ketoacidosis in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Lancet 1997; 350:1505–1510Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Paulus MP: Decision-making dysfunctions in psychiatry—altered homeostatic processing? Science 2007; 318:602–606Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 McNally K, Rohan J, Pendley JS, et al.: Executive functioning, treatment adherence, and glycemic control in children with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2010; 33:1159–1162Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Raichle ME: Neuropsychology. The scratchpad of the mind. Nature 1993; 363:583–584Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Waldrop-Valverde D, Jones DL, Gould F, et al.: Neurocognition, health-related reading literacy, and numeracy in medication management for HIV infection. AIDS Patient Care STDS 2010; 24:477–484Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Schreiner B, Brow S, Phillips M: Management Strategies for the Adolescent Lifestyle. Diabetes Spectrum 2000; 13:83–87Google Scholar

9 Ancker JS, Kaufman D: Rethinking health numeracy: a multidisciplinary literature review. J Am Med Inform Assoc 2007; 14:713–721Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Hirsch IB, Farkas-Hirsch R, Skyler JS: Intensive insulin therapy for treatment of type I diabetes. Diabetes Care 1990; 13:1265–1283Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Kodl CT, Seaquist ER: Cognitive dysfunction and diabetes mellitus. Endocr Rev 2008; 29:494–511Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Doya K: Modulators of decision-making. Nat Neurosci 2008; 11:410–416Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Ramakrishnan R, Sheeladevi R, Suthanthirarajan N: PKC-α mediated alterations of indoleamine contents in diabetic rat brain. Brain Res Bull 2004; 64:189–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Gallego M, Setién R, Izquierdo MJ, et al.: Diabetes-induced biochemical changes in central and peripheral catecholaminergic systems. Physiol Res 2003; 52:735–741MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Bellush LL, Reid SG: Altered behavior and neurochemistry during short-term insulin withdrawal in streptozocin-induced diabetic rats. Diabetes 1991; 40:217–222Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Coyle JT, Puttfarcken P: Oxidative stress, glutamate, and neurodegenerative disorders. Science 1993; 262:689–695Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Wysocki T, Iannotti R, Weissberg-Benchell J, et al.Family Management of Childhood Diabetes Steering Committee: Diabetes problem-solving by youths with type 1 diabetes and their caregivers: measurement, validation, and longitudinal associations with glycemic control. J Pediatr Psychol 2008; 33:875–884Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Hill-Briggs F, Gemmell L: Problem solving in diabetes self-management and control: a systematic review of the literature. Diabetes Educ 2007; 33:1032–1050, discussion 1051–1052Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Miller DC, Byrnes JP: To achieve or not to achieve: a self-regulation perspective on adolescents’ academic decision-making. J Educ Psychol 2001; 93:677–685CrossrefGoogle Scholar

20 Schlundt DG, Flannery ME, Davis DL, et al.: Evaluation of a multicomponent, behaviorally oriented, problem-based “summer school” program for adolescents with diabetes. Behav Modif 1999; 23:79–105Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Miller VA, Drotar D: Decision-making competence and adherence to treatment in adolescents with diabetes. J Pediatr Psychol 2007; 32:178–188Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Mann L, Burnett P, Radford M, et al.: The Melbourne Decision-Making Questionnaire: an instrument for measuring patterns for coping with decisional conflict. J Behav Decis Making 1997; 10:1–19CrossrefGoogle Scholar

23 Popoviç M, Biessels GJ, Isaacson RL, et al.: Learning and memory in streptozotocin-induced diabetic rats in a novel spatial/object discrimination task. Behav Brain Res 2001; 122:201–207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Kamal A, Biessels GJ, Urban IJ, et al.: Hippocampal synaptic plasticity in streptozotocin-diabetic rats: impairment of long-term potentiation and facilitation of long-term depression. Neuroscience 1999; 90:737–745Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Biessels GJ, Kamal A, Ramakers GM, et al.: Place learning and hippocampal synaptic plasticity in streptozotocin-induced diabetic rats. Diabetes 1996; 45:1259–1266Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Flood JF, Mooradian AD, Morley JE: Characteristics of learning and memory in streptozocin-induced diabetic mice. Diabetes 1990; 39:1391–1398Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Biessels GJ, Kamal A, Urban IJA, et al.: Water maze learning and hippocampal synaptic plasticity in streptozotocin-diabetic rats: effects of insulin treatment. Brain Res 1998; 800:125–135Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 McIntyre RS, Soczynska JK, Konarski JZ, et al.: Should depressive syndromes be reclassified as “metabolic syndrome Type II”? Ann Clin Psychiatry 2007; 19:257–264Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Klein JP, Waxman SG: The brain in diabetes: molecular changes in neurons and their implications for end-organ damage. Lancet Neurol 2003; 2:548–554Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Woods SC, Benoit SC, Clegg DJ: The Brain-Gut Islet Connection. Diabetes 2006; 55(supplement 2):S114–S121CrossrefGoogle Scholar

31 Brands AMA, Biessels GJ, de Haan EHF, et al.: The effects of type 1 diabetes on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Diabetes Care 2005; 28:726–735Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Ryan CM, Williams TM, Orchard TJ, et al.: Psychomotor slowing is associated with distal symmetrical polyneuropathy in adults with diabetes mellitus. Diabetes 1992; 41:107–113Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Lawson JS, Erdahl DL, Monga TN, et al.: Neuropsychological function in diabetic patients with neuropathy. Br J Psychiatry 1984; 145:263–268Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Ryan CM, Williams TM: Effects of insulin-dependent diabetes on learning and memory efficiency in adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1993; 15:685–700Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Franceschi M, Cecchetto R, Minicucci F, et al.: Cognitive processes in insulin-dependent diabetes. Diabetes Care 1984; 7:228–231Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Skenazy JA, Bigler ED: Neuropsychological findings in diabetes mellitus. J Clin Psychol 1984; 40:246–258Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Kramer L, Fasching P, Madl C, et al.: Previous episodes of hypoglycemic coma are not associated with permanent cognitive brain dysfunction in IDDM patients on intensive insulin treatment. Diabetes 1998; 47:1909–1914Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Prescott JH, Richardson JT, Gillespie CR: Cognitive function in diabetes mellitus: the effects of duration of illness and glycaemic control. Br J Clin Psychol 1990; 29:167–175Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Stiles MC, Seaquist ER: Cerebral structural and functional changes in type 1 diabetes. Minerva Med 2010; 101:105–114MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Bolo NR, Musen G, Jacobson AM, et al.: Brain activation during working memory is altered in patients with type 1 diabetes during hypoglycemia. Diabetes 2011; 60:3256–3264Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Hernandez CA, Bradish GI, Laschinger HKS, et al.: Self-awareness work in Type 1 diabetes: Traversing experience and negotiating collaboration. Canadian Journal of Diabetes Care 1997; 21:21–27Google Scholar

42 Paterson BL, Sloan J: A phenomenological study of the decision-making experience of individuals with long-standing diabetes. Canadian Journal of Diabetes Care 1994; 18:10–19Google Scholar

43 Cox DJ, Gonder-Frederick L, Ritterband L, et al.: Blood glucose awareness training: what is it, where is it, and where is it going? Diabetes Spectrum 2006; 19:43–49CrossrefGoogle Scholar

44 Weinger K, Jacobson AM, Draelos MT, et al.: Blood glucose estimation and symptoms during hyperglycemia and hypoglycemia in patients with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Am J Med 1995; 98:22–31Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Meltzer LJ, Johnson SB, Pappachan S, et al.: Blood glucose estimations in adolescents with type 1 diabetes: predictors of accuracy and error. J Pediatr Psychol 2003; 28:203–211Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Bechara A: The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn 2004; 55:30–40Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, et al.: Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 1997; 275:1293–1295Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, et al.: The Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some questions and answers. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9:159–162, discussion 162–164Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Emotion, decision-making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2000; 10:295–307Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Sanfey AG, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, et al.: Neuroeconomics: cross-currents in research on decision-making. Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10:108–116Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51 Cryer PE: Mechanisms of hypoglycemia-associated autonomic failure and its component syndromes in diabetes. Diabetes 2005; 54:3592–3601Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Heller SR, Cryer PE: Reduced neuroendocrine and symptomatic responses to subsequent hypoglycemia in nondiabetic humans. Diabetes 1991; 40:223–226Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

53 George E, Marques JL, Harris ND, et al.: Preservation of physiological responses to hypoglycemia 2 days after antecedent hypoglycemia in patients with IDDM. Diabetes Care 1997; 20:1293–1298Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54 Dagogo-Jack SE, Craft S, Cryer PE: Hypoglycemia-associated autonomic failure in insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus: recent antecedent hypoglycemia reduces autonomic responses to, symptoms of, and defense against subsequent hypoglycemia. J Clin Invest 1993; 91:819–828Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

55 Dunn JT, Cranston I, Marsden PK, et al.: Attenuation of amygdala and frontal cortical responses to low blood glucose concentration in asymptomatic hypoglycemia in type 1 diabetes: a new player in hypoglycemia unawareness? Diabetes 2007; 56:2766–2773Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Cranston IC, Reed LJ, Marsden PK, et al.: Changes in regional brain (18)F-fluorodeoxyglucose uptake at hypoglycemia in type 1 diabetic men associated with hypoglycemia unawareness and counter-regulatory failure. Diabetes 2001; 50:2329–2336Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Armario A, Valles A, Dal-Zotto S, et al.: A single exposure to severe stressors causes long-term desensitization of the physiological response to the homotypic stressor. Stress 2004; 7:152–172CrossrefGoogle Scholar

58 Smith CB, Choudhary P, Pernet A, et al.: Hypoglycemia unawareness is associated with reduced adherence to therapeutic decisions in patients with type 1 diabetes: evidence from a clinical audit. Diabetes Care 2009; 32:1196–1198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59 Leventhal H, Diefenbach M, Leventhal EA: Illness cognition: using common sense to understand treatment adherence and affect cognition interactions. Cogn Ther Res 1992; 16:143–163CrossrefGoogle Scholar

60 Zammitt NN, Warren RE, Deary IJ, et al.: Delayed recovery of cognitive function following hypoglycemia in adults with type 1 diabetes: effect of impaired awareness of hypoglycemia. Diabetes 2008; 57:732–736Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61 Hilsted J: Cognitive function in hypoglycemia unawareness. International Diabetes Monitor 2009; 21:33–34Google Scholar

62 Böber E, Büyükgebiz A, Verrotti A, et al.: Hypoglycemia, hypoglycemia unawareness, and counterregulation in children and adolescents with type 1 diabetes mellitus. J Pediatr Endocrinol Metab 2005; 18:831–841Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63 Ly TT, Hewitt J, Davey RJ, et al.: Improving epinephrine responses in hypoglycemia unawareness with real-time continuous glucose monitoring in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2011; 34:50–52Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

64 Barnard KD, Skinner TC, Peveler R: The prevalence of comorbid depression in adults with Type 1 diabetes: systematic literature review. Diabet Med 2006; 23:445–448Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

65 Gendelman N, Snell-Bergeon JK, McFann K, et al.: Prevalence and correlates of depression in individuals with and without type 1 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2009; 32:575–579Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66 Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK: Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX, Psychological Corporation, 1996Google Scholar

67 van Randenborgh A, de Jong-Meyer R, Hüffmeier J: Decision-making in depression: differences in decisional conflict between healthy and depressed individuals. Clin Psychol Psychother 2010; 17:285–298MedlineGoogle Scholar

68 Shad MU, Bidesi AP, Chen LA, et al.: Neurobiology of decision-making in depressed adolescents: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 50:612–621, e2Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

69 Forbes EE, Shaw DS, Dahl RE: Alterations in reward-related decision-making in boys with recent and future depression. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:633–639Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

70 Rogers RD, Tunbridge EM, Bhagwagar Z, et al.: Tryptophan depletion alters the decision-making of healthy volunteers through altered processing of reward cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003; 28:153–162Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

71 Rogers RD, Ramnani N, Mackay C, et al.: Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:594–602Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

72 Cella M, Dymond S, Cooper A: Impaired flexible decision-making in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord 2010; 124:207–210Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73 Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, et al.: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 1994; 50:7–15Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

74 Turnbull OH, Evans CE, Kemish K, et al.: A novel set-shifting modification of the Iowa Gambling Task: flexible emotion-based learning in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 2006; 20:290–298Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

75 Dymond S, Cella M, Cooper A, et al.: The contingency-shifting variant Iowa Gambling Task: an investigation with young adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2010; 32:239–248Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

76 Forbes EE, Christopher May J, Siegle GJ, et al.: Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: an fMRI study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:1031–1040Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

77 Ernst M, Nelson EE, McClure EB, et al.: Choice selection and reward anticipation: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2004; 42:1585–1597Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

78 Mayberg HS: Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997; 9:471–481LinkGoogle Scholar

79 Rogers RD, Everitt BJ, Baldacchino A, et al.: Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers: evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology 1999; 20:322–339Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar