The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Clinical and Research ReportsFull Access

Vicarious Embarrassment or “Fremdscham”: Overendorsement in Frontotemporal Dementia

Abstract

Objective:

The experience of embarrassment signals violations in social norms, and impairment in this social emotion may underlie much of the social dysfunction in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). The authors investigated whether impaired self-awareness of embarrassment may distinguish patients with bvFTD early in the course of disease from healthy control subjects (HCs).

Methods:

Self-reported embarrassment was examined among 18 patients with early bvFTD and 23 HCs by using the 36-item Embarrassability Scale, which includes items of situations eliciting embarrassment for oneself (“self-embarrassment”) and embarrassment for others (“vicarious embarrassment”). The two study groups were also compared with the Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ). The analyses included correlations of SNQ results (total score, violations or “break” errors, and overendorsement of social rules or “overadhere” errors) with Embarrassability Scale scores.

Results:

Patients with bvFTD did not differ from HCs on total or self-embarrassment scores but did have significantly higher vicarious embarrassment scores. Unlike in the HC group, reports of vicarious embarrassment did not differ from reports of self-embarrassment among patients in the bvFTD group. The Embarrassability Score further correlated with overadherence to norms on the SNQ.

Conclusions:

In the presence of social dysfunction and emotional blunting, these findings suggest that patients with bvFTD rely on their own perspective for a rule-based application of social norms in reporting vicarious embarrassment. The assessment of reports of embarrassment for others may indicate an early and previously unrecognized clinical measure for detecting bvFTD.

Behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) is predominantly a disorder of socioemotional behavior (1). This neurodegenerative disease involves frontal and anterior temporal lobes resulting in disturbances in social interactions and emotional blunting (2). Investigators have characterized patients with bvFTD as being particularly impaired in self-conscious emotions, which are necessary for the type of feedback that promotes social behavior (3, 4). These include social emotions such as shame, guilt, pride, and, most prominently, the ability to experience embarrassment.

Embarrassment is emotional discomfort associated with perceived social disapproval for violating a social norm (5). Self-embarrassment requires examining one’s behavior through the eyes of others, knowing the social norm violation, and self-appraisal for corrective or reparative actions. It is most intense when the disapproval is directed at the self, but embarrassment may also be experienced vicariously when perceiving social disapproval directed at another; this embarrassment-by-proxy is sometimes referred to by its German name “fremdscham” (6).

bvFTD prominently impairs embarrassment and other self-conscious emotions (7). Patients with bvFTD have decreased emotional reactivity to embarrassing stimuli, such as watching themselves singing (1), decreased self-consciousness (4), and impaired self-awareness from others’ perspective or self-referential processing (3, 4, 812). Despite reactivity to simple happy and sad emotional films and facial emotional responses to auditory startle (4, 13), patients in early stages of bvFTD display fewer facial signs of embarrassment than control subjects (4). Finally, bvFTD patients do not show embarrassment and corresponding autonomic reactivity when recognizing their own errors or mistakes (14).

A decreased ability to experience embarrassment may be one of the most basic and early features of bvFTD. Patients with bvFTD are known to have emotional blunting, decreased empathy, and impaired self-referential emotions (3, 4, 7, 13, 15). They exhibit impairment in experiencing embarrassment on an emotional level, although they are able to report embarrassment on a cognitive level (i.e., on the basis of what they believe should be felt) (16). These patients also lack general emotional awareness, often react inappropriately to social norms, and experience difficulties in taking the perspective of others or in mentalization (“theory of mind”) (1719). All of these deficits may further impair self-awareness of embarrassment. Consequently, an assessment of self-reports of embarrassment in social situations may be a highly sensitive measure for detecting early bvFTD, a disorder for which a definitive clinical test is lacking.

In the present study, we evaluated self-perceived embarrassment among patients with bvFTD compared with normal control subjects. Participants were evaluated with the Embarrassability Scale, containing 36 items equally divided between self and vicarious embarrassment. Participants’ performance on the Embarrassability Scale was compared with their performance on the Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ), which measures the breaking of or overadherence to social rules as part of the Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration Module of the National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center database (20).

Methods

Participants

Participants with bvFTD were recruited from the Behavioral Neurology Program and Clinic at the University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), where they underwent clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging assessments. Age-matched healthy control subjects (HCs) were volunteers recruited from the community. Under approval by the institutional review board at UCLA, 41 participants (patients with bvFTD, N=18; HCs, N=23) were enrolled in this study. Informed consent was obtained from participants and caregivers. Patients with bvFTD met International Consensus Criteria for clinically probable bvFTD (2). Clinical diagnoses of bvFTD were supported by predominant frontal and anterior temporal involvement on MRI and/or fluoro-deoxyglucose positron emission tomography brain scans. Exclusion criteria included the presence of complicating medical or psychiatric illnesses or psychoactive medication.

Procedures

Patients with bvFTD were evaluated with four social and emotional scales. Two of these scales were self-administered in both study groups (bvFTD and HC): the modified UCLA Embarrassability Scale (21) and the Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ) (22, 23). Two caregiver-administered scales were completed by the caregivers of bvFTD patients: the Social Dysfunction Scale (SDS) (24) and the Scale for Emotional Blunting (SEB) (15).

Self-Administered Scales

  1. The UCLA version of the Embarrassability Scale, which was modified from Modigliani’s original scale (21), is a significantly expanded and modernized instrument for contemporary use in the United States. It is a self-administered instrument containing 36 items divided into 18 self-embarrassing situations and 18 items depicting situations that are embarrassing for others (vicarious items). Participants are asked to imagine these situations as vividly as possible and whether the situation would cause the participant embarrassment, including self-consciousness, awkwardness, discomfort, or a sense of social exposure. They are told that some items may involve feeling embarrassed for oneself and that other items may involve feeling embarrassed for someone else. They are then asked to record their level of embarrassment on a 5-point Likert scale, with a score of 1 indicating “I would not feel the least embarrassed: not awkward or uncomfortable at all” and a score of 5 indicating “I would feel strongly embarrassed: extremely self-conscious, awkward, and uncomfortable.” Examples of items of self-embarrassment are as follows: “Suppose you go to pay at a restaurant and find that you did not bring your purse or wallet”; and “Suppose that, while laughing heartily with a group of friends, you pass gas.” Examples of items of vicarious embarrassment are as follows: “Suppose you observe someone go into a bathroom and emerge with toilet paper sticking to his shoes”; and “Suppose you are watching an amateur comedy show and one of the performers is unable to make anyone laugh with her jokes.”

  2. The SNQ is a 22-item “yes” or “no” questionnaire administered to participants to detect inappropriate social behavior in hypothetical scenarios (22, 23). The SNQ is recommended as part of the socioemotional evaluation of patients with bvFTD (20). Written instructions are as follows: “The following is a list of behaviors that a person might engage in. Please decide whether or not it would be socially acceptable and appropriate to do these things in the mainstream culture of the United States and answer yes or no to each. Think about these questions as if they were occurring in front of or with a stranger or acquaintance, not a close friend or family member.” For example, “would it be socially acceptable to wear the same shirt twice in two weeks?” A total score is obtained by summing correct items, with higher scores indicating greater knowledge of social norms. There are subscales that measure two types of errors: break errors refer to endorsement of a socially inappropriate behavior (e.g., eating pasta with your fingers) as appropriate, and overadhere errors refer to endorsement of a socially appropriate behavior (e.g., wearing the same shirt twice in two weeks) as inappropriate. Although used successfully to distinguish patients with bvFTD and other patient populations (22, 23), the reliability of the SNQ is not established.

bvFTD Caregiver-Administered Scales

  1. The SDS is a 40-item informant-based rating scale previously used to characterize social behavioral disturbances in bvFTD (24). The SDS is completed by a spouse, family member, caregiver, or other informant who knows the patient well. The scale originates from the Social Competency Questionnaire, a measure of adaptive social behaviors, with modifications for use with patients with bvFTD. Written instructions are as follows: “For each item, base ratings on a comparison with typical behavior before disease symptoms emerged.” Informants rate items regarding the patient’s social behavior on a 5-point Likert scale (1=very inaccurate, 2=somewhat inaccurate, 3=neither accurate nor inaccurate, 4=somewhat accurate, 5=very accurate). One example of a questionnaire item is as follows: “Makes inappropriate comments to others.” The 40 items are summed, yielding a total raw score, with higher scores suggestive of greater social dysfunction. Previous work has shown high internal consistency and reliability for the SDS (Cronbach’s α=0.977) (24).

  2. Caregivers complete the SEB, an instrument used successfully to evaluate patients with bvFTD (25). This scale queries domains such as absence of pleasure-seeking behavior (behavior), affective blunting (affect), and cognitive blunting (thought). Each behavioral symptom is rated by indicating the following: “condition absent” (0 points), “slightly present or doubtful” (1 point), or “clearly present” (2 points). The behavior subscale items include symptoms akin to reclusivity or avoiding social contact. The affect domain involves behaviors such as lacking warmth or empathy. The thought subscale includes items regarding lacking plans, ambition, desires, or drive. The interrater reliability coefficient for this instrument has been robust in previous investigations (α=0.91) (15).

Disease Severity

Disease severity of patients with bvFTD was assessed with confirmation of disease duration and by using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (26), the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) (27), and the Functional Assessment Questionnaire (FAQ) (28).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted with SPSS version 25.0. Demographic, descriptive characteristics were generated for both study groups (bvFTD and HC) and compared by using chi-square and t tests for categorical and continuous variables, respectively. The null hypothesis that data for the two major scales (Embarrassability Scale and SNQ) were normally distributed was rejected when testing for distribution of data with the Shapiro-Wilk test. Mann-Whitney U tests were subsequently used for comparison of means between the two study groups. Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used for within-group evaluation of embarrassability subscale scores. Across both groups, Spearman’s rank-order correlations were computed to examine the relationship between the two self-administered behavioral scales.

Results

Participant Characteristics

There were no significant group differences on age, sex, or years of education (Table 1). Patients with bvFTD were, on average, about 4 years into their disease from the time of the first-onset of symptoms and had MMSE and MoCA scores in the mild range, despite moderate functional impairment on the FAQ. Patients’ level of cognitive impairment did not preclude understanding and completion of the self-administered scales.

TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and healthy control subjects (HCs)a

CharacteristicbvFTD (N=18)HC (N=23)
MeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)61.2510.156.338.16
Education (years)15.922.4316.091.78
Disease duration (years)4.133.72
Mini-Mental State Examination23.674.65
Montreal Cognitive Assessment17.336.49
Functional Assessment Questionnaire totalb19.36.50
Socioemotional Dysfunction Scalec142.2131.22
Scale for Emotional Bluntingd12.259.26

aThe male:female ratios for the bvFTD and HC groups were 9:9 and 11:12, respectively.

bScores >9 indicate functional impairment (range, 0–30).

cScores >105 indicate social dysfunction (range, 40–200).

dScores >12 indicate emotional dysfunction (range 0–32).

TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and healthy control subjects (HCs)a

Enlarge table

Behavioral Assessments

Caregiver-administered scales.

The bvFTD group, compared with previously reported norms (15, 24), exhibited social dysfunction and emotional blunting on the SDS and SEB, respectively (Table 1).

Self-administered scales.

Patients with bvFTD did not differ significantly from HCs on the Embarrassability Scale total score (Table 2). On the subscale measures, there were no group differences in self-embarrassment; however, bvFTD patients endorsed greater vicarious embarrassment (p=0.032) compared with HCs. In the bvFTD group, patients did not report significantly greater embarrassability for items involving self versus others, yet the HC group had significantly more self, than other, embarrassment (p<0.001).

TABLE 2. Results of the Embarrassability Scale and Social Norms Questionnaire among patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and healthy control subjects (HCs)a

MeasurebvFTD (N=18)HC (N=23)p
MeanSDMeanSD
Embarrassability total score110.1732.61100.023.64n.s.
Self-embarrassment subscore56.2817.6857.1311.66n.s.
Vicarious embarrassment subscore53.8917.8842.8714.860.032
Social Norms Questionnaire score15.932.8919.861.64<0.001
Break error score1.531.340.860.89n.s.
Overadherence error score4.542.771.271.45<0.001

aThe data represent independent samples with differences analyzed with the Mann-Whitney U test. n.s.=not significant.

TABLE 2. Results of the Embarrassability Scale and Social Norms Questionnaire among patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and healthy control subjects (HCs)a

Enlarge table

Norm, break, and overadherence errors.

Patients with bvFTD had more social norm errors compared with HCs on the SNQ (p<0.001). On the subscale measures, there were no group differences in SNQ break errors; however, bvFTD patients made more overadherence errors on the SNQ (p<0.001) compared with HCs. Correlational analysis showed significant Spearman’s rho correlations between the total Embarrassability Scale score and SNQ overadherence errors (p<0.05); correlations fell just short of significance between vicarious embarrassment and overadherence errors (p=0.056) (Table 3).

TABLE 3. Correlations of the Embarrassability Scale and Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ) across groupsa

SNQEmbarrassability Scale total scoreSelf-embarrassment subscoreVicarious embarrassment subscore
Spearman’s rhopSpearman’s rhopSpearman’s rhop
Total score0.1870.2970.0530.7700.210.236
Break subscore0.1990.2660.3120.0770.1240.490
Overadhere subscore0.3590.040.2890.1030.3360.056

aThe p values are presented at two-tailed significance levels.

TABLE 3. Correlations of the Embarrassability Scale and Social Norms Questionnaire (SNQ) across groupsa

Enlarge table

Discussion

Decreased ability to be embarrassed may underlie many of the social behavioral disturbances among patients with bvFTD, and an assessment of their awareness of embarrassment could aid in early recognition of this disorder. We investigated self-reports of embarrassment among patients with bvFTD compared with HCs. Patients did not differ from HCs on the total Embarrassability Scale score or on embarrassing items involving themselves; however, compared with HCs, patients reported experiencing significantly greater embarrassment for items involving embarrassing situations for others (vicarious). In comparison, caregivers rated bvFTD patients as having social dysfunction and emotional blunting, and the patients rated themselves worse than HCs in responding to social norms. Finally, results from the Embarrassment Scale correlated with overadherence to social norms. Together, these findings suggest that among patients with bvFTD, increased endorsement of vicarious embarrassment may reflect an inability to take another’s perspective, resulting in rule-based responses regarding levels of embarrassment.

Social emotions, such as embarrassment, are distinct from basic emotions (5); in particular, they require the ability to view oneself and others from the perspective of others (2931). Self-embarrassment involves self-appraisal of adherence to social norms as seen by others, prompting corrective or reparative actions when necessary (5, 3242). However, embarrassment can also be a vicarious experience when perceiving the embarrassing actions or mistakes of others (6, 43). In observing someone else violating social norms with potential for social disapproval (44), people imagine themselves from the other’s perspective. Vicarious embarrassment, or fremdscham, is distinct from emotional contagion because it does not require observing the other person’s emotional reaction or even the presence of third parties (43). Both forms of embarrassment rely on taking the perspective of others, an aspect of theory of mind, and assessing violations of social norm rules.

Self-conscious emotions, such as embarrassment, involve the frontal, temporal and limbic areas affected by bvFTD (36, 41, 42, 4548). Frontopolar, ventromedial frontal, and basal forebrain regions are involved in prosocial sentiments such as embarrassment (47). In an embarrassing task in which bvFTD patients watched themselves singing karaoke, there was decreased physiological and behavioral reactivity associated with smaller right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex gray matter volumes (49). Situations that trigger vicarious embarrassment involve brain areas associated with pain (either physical or social), such as the anterior cingulate cortex and the left anterior insula (43, 50). These are areas affected early in the course of bvFTD.

Given the lack of self-conscious emotions and actual feelings of embarrassment in bvFTD, it is noteworthy that these patients report more vicarious embarrassment than HCs. There appear to be several reasons for this. First, patients with bvFTD have deficits in mentalization or theory of mind, the basic yardstick for taking the perspective of others (18). Hence, their perspective is from their own point of view. Second, the claim of increased embarrassment is associated with overadherence errors to social norms. Increased overadherence errors in bvFTD correlate with difficulty recognizing the changing context of a rule (22), and bvFTD facilitates rule-based and utilitarian judgments on the basis of previously learned social rules (51, 52). Consequently, even in reporting self-embarrassment, patients with bvFTD may be responding less to actual social discomfort and more to what they perceive as an expected level of embarrassment. They may be applying the same predetermined rule for the level of self-embarrassment per social norm violation to the reporting of embarrassment for others.

There are several alternative, but less plausible, explanations for the results in the present study. The patients with bvFTD could have had a truly increased sense of vicarious embarrassment. This interpretation, however, is inconsistent with the vast body of research on socioemotional impairments in bvFTD (3, 4, 7, 13, 15), including the basic diagnostic criteria of loss of empathy or sympathy (2). A second consideration is that for vicarious items, bvFTD patients reported the perceived embarrassment or social discomfort of others rather than their own. However, this seems very unlikely given the clear task instructions and vicarious reports, as well as the impaired mentalization and perspective taking among patients with bvFTD (1719). Another consideration is that in this study, we could not entirely exclude an effect on the results from the limited, established reliability and other psychometric aspects of the Embarrassability Scale and other measures.

There are several limitations to this study that could be addressed in future investigations. First, there is the consideration of the small number of study subjects, especially in the bvFTD group. The number of participants was sufficient to disclose group differences on some scales but not on others. Second, it would have been beneficial to include a direct measure of mentalization or theory of mind. Third, it would also have been advantageous to have had autonomic measures for assessing a physiological reaction of embarrassment. Fourth, the modified Embarrassability Scale has not been broadly validated in a large population. Finally, other control groups, such as patients with Alzheimer’s disease, were not included.

Conclusions

These results show that patients with bvFTD, despite known impairment in experiencing embarrassment, report significantly more vicarious embarrassment compared with normal control subjects, and this vicarious embarrassment appears to be associated with overadherence to social norms. In addition, these results suggest a self-centered, rule-based reasoning in response to questions of embarrassment for others. Given the social behavioral changes in patients with bvFTD, and the relative insensitivity of traditional neuropsychological measures for early bvFTD, a self-rating scale of vicarious embarrassment could be useful in the initial recognition of this disorder.

The Departments of Neurology (Mendez, Yerstein, Jimenez) and Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences (Mendez), David Geffen School of Medicine, University of California at Los Angeles; and the Neurobehavior Unit, VA Greater Los Angeles Healthcare System, Los Angeles (Mendez, Yerstein, Jimenez).
Send correspondence to Dr. Mendez ().

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supported by NIH (grants R01AG034499-05 and 1RF1AG050967-01A1).

References

1 Barsuglia JP, Nedjat-Haiem FR, Shapira JS, et al.: Observational themes of social behavioral disturbances in frontotemporal dementia. Int Psychogeriatr 2014; 20:1–7Google Scholar

2 Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, Knopman D, et al.: Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 2011; 134:2456–2477Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Sturm VE, Ascher EA, Miller BL, et al.: Diminished self-conscious emotional responding in frontotemporal lobar degeneration patients. Emotion 2008; 8:861–869Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Sturm VE, Rosen HJ, Allison S, et al.: Self-conscious emotion deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Brain 2006; 129:2508–2516Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Keltner D, Buswell BN: Embarrassment: its distinct form and appeasement functions. Psychol Bull 1997; 122:250–270Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Paulus FM, Muller-Pinzler L, Stolz DS, et al.: Laugh or cringe? common and distinct processes of reward-based schadenfreude and empathy-based fremdscham. Neuropsychologia 2018; 116(Pt A):52–60Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Goodkind MS, Sturm VE, Ascher EA, et al.: Emotion recognition in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: a new film-based assessment. Emotion 2015; 15:416–427Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Shany-Ur T, Lin N, Rosen HJ, et al.: Self-awareness in neurodegenerative disease relies on neural structures mediating reward-driven attention. Brain 2014; 137:2368–2381Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Rankin KP, Baldwin E, Pace-Savitsky C, et al.: Self awareness and personality change in dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005; 76:632–639Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Williamson C, Alcantar O, Rothlind J, et al.: Standardised measurement of self-awareness deficits in FTD and AD. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2010; 81:140–145Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Arroyo-Anlló EM, Bouston AT, Fargeau MN, et al.: Self-consciousness in patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 49:1021–1029Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Massimo L, Libon DJ, Chandrasekaran K, et al.: Self-appraisal in behavioural variant frontotemporal degeneration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013; 84:148–153Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Eckart JA, Sturm VE, Miller BL, et al.: Diminished disgust reactivity in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50:786–790Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Scherling CS, Zakrzewski J, Datta S, et al.: Mistakes, too few to mention? impaired self-conscious emotional processing of errors in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Front Behav Neurosci 2017; 11:189Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Mendez MF, McMurtray A, Licht E, et al.: The scale for emotional blunting in patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase 2006; 12:242–246Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Fong SS, Navarrete CD, Perfecto SE, et al.: Behavioral and autonomic reactivity to moral dilemmas in frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s disease. Soc Neurosci 2017; 12:409–418Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Mendez MF, Shapira JS: Loss of emotional insight in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia or “frontal anosodiaphoria”. Conscious Cogn 2011; 20:1690–1696Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Adenzato M, Cavallo M, Enrici I: Theory of mind ability in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: an analysis of the neural, cognitive, and social levels. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48:2–12Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Dermody N, Wong S, Ahmed R, et al.: Uncovering the neural bases of cognitive and affective empathy deficits in Alzheimer’s disease and the behavioral-variant of frontotemporal dementia. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 53:801–816Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Kukull WA: The NACC Database. 2010; https://www.alz.washington.edu/WEB/about_about.htmlGoogle Scholar

21 Modigliani A: Embarrassment and embarrassability. Sociometry 1968; 31:313–326Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Panchal H, Paholpak P, Lee G, et al.: Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical correlates of the Social Norms Questionnaire in frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 2016; 31:326–332Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Ganguli M, Sun Z, McDade E, et al.: That’s inappropriate! social norms in an older population-based cohort. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2018; 32:150–155Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Barsuglia JP, Kaiser NC, Wilkins SS, et al.: A scale of socioemotional dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2014; 29:793–805Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Abrams R, Taylor MA: A rating scale for emotional blunting. Am J Psychiatry 1978; 135:226–229Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al.: The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53:695–699Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Pfeffer RI, Kurosaki TT, Harrah CH Jr, et al.: Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol 1982; 37:323–329Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Lewis M, Sullivan WM: The development of self-conscious emotions, in Handbook of Competence and Motivation. Edited by Elliott AJ, Dweck CS. New York, Guilford Press, 2005, pp 185–201Google Scholar

30 Leary MR: Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Annu Rev Psychol 2007; 58:317–344Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Tangney JP, Tracy JL: Self-conscious emotions, in Handbook of Self and Identity, 2nd ed. Edited by Leary MR, Tangney JP. New York, Guilford Press, 2013, pp 446–479Google Scholar

32 Muris P, Meesters C, Bouwman L, et al.: Relations among behavioral inhibition, shame- and guilt-proneness, and anxiety disorders symptoms in non-clinical children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2015; 46:209–216Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Tracey LJ, Robins RW: The self in self-conscious emotions: a cognitive appraisal approach, in The Self-Conscious Emotions Theory and Research. Edited by Tracy JL, Robins RW, Tangney JP. New York, Guilford Press, 2007, p 3–20Google Scholar

34 Tracy JL, Robins RW: Self-conscious emotions: where self and emotion meet, in Frontiers of Social Psychology: The Self. Edited by Sedikides C, Spencer SJ, 2007, New York, Psychology Press, pp 187–209Google Scholar

35 Tracy JL, Robins RW: Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model. Psychol Inq 2004; 15:103–125CrossrefGoogle Scholar

36 Gilead M, Katzir M, Eyal T, et al.: Neural correlates of processing “self-conscious” vs “basic” emotions. Neuropsychologia 2016; 81:207–218Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Muris P, Meesters C: Small or big in the eyes of the other: on the developmental psychopathology of self-conscious emotions as shame, guilt, and pride. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2014; 17:19–40Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Tangney JP, Stuewig J, Mashek DJ: Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 2007; 58:345–372Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Muris P, Meesters C, Heijmans J, et al.: Lack of guilt, guilt, and shame: a multi-informant study on the relations between self-conscious emotions and psychopathology in clinically referred children and adolescents. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016; 25:383–396Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Chang LJ, Smith A, Dufwenberg M, et al.: Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversion. Neuron 2011; 70:560–572Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Beer JS: Neural systems for self-conscious emotions and their underlying appraisals, in The Self-Conscious Emotions Theory and Research. Edited by Tracy JL, Robins RW, Tangney JP. New York, Guilford Press 2007, pp 53–67Google Scholar

42 Jankowski KF, Takahashi H: Cognitive neuroscience of social emotions and implications for psychopathology: examining embarrassment, guilt, envy, and schadenfreude. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2014; 68:319–336Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Krach S, Cohrs JC, de Echeverría Loebell NC, et al.: Your flaws are my pain: linking empathy to vicarious embarrassment. PLoS One 2011; 6:e18675Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Paulus FM, Müller-Pinzler L, Jansen A, et al.: Mentalizing and the role of the posterior superior temporal sulcus in sharing others’ embarrassment. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25:2065–2075Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

45 Berthoz S, Armony JL, Blair RJ, et al.: An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain 2002; 125:1696–1708Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Beer JS, Lombardo MV, Bhanji JP: Roles of medial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex in self-evaluation. J Cogn Neurosci 2010; 22:2108–2119Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Moll J, Zahn R, de Oliveira-Souza R, et al.: Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage 2011; 54:1735–1742Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Takahashi H, Yahata N, Koeda M, et al.: Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2004; 23:967–974Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Sturm VE, Sollberger M, Seeley WW, et al.: Role of right pregenual anterior cingulate cortex in self-conscious emotional reactivity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013; 8:468–474Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

50 Krach S, Kamp-Becker I, Einhäuser W, et al.: Evidence from pupillometry and fMRI indicates reduced neural response during vicarious social pain but not physical pain in autism. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36:4730–4744Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

51 Carr AR, Paholpak P, Daianu M, et al.: An investigation of care-based vs. rule-based morality in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, and healthy controls. Neuropsychologia 2015; 78:73–79Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

52 Mendez MF, Shapira JS: Altered emotional morality in frontotemporal dementia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2009; 14:165–179Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar