The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Clinical Research ReportsFull Access

Disorganized Symptoms and Executive Functioning Predict Impaired Social Functioning in Subjects at Risk for Psychosis

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.23.4.jnp457

Abstract

Predictors of social functioning deficits were assessed in 22 individuals “at risk” for psychosis. Disorganized symptoms and executive functioning predicted social functioning at follow-up. Early intervention efforts that focus on social and cognitive skills are indicated in this vulnerable population.

Schizophrenia is a chronic illness that manifests in late adolescence or early adulthood and is often preceded by both a premorbid and a prodromal phase. Unlike the premorbid phase, which is a period of relatively stable social and cognitive deficits,1 the prodromal period is characterized by its lack of stability and a downhill course of psychosocial impairment, culminating in the onset of frank psychosis.2,3

Poor social functioning at initial assessment has been implicated as an ominous sign in subjects at risk for psychosis. It has been shown that decline in social functioning before ascertainment is associated with later conversion to psychosis.4

Dysfunction in multiple cognitive domains has been reported in patients with schizophrenia as well as in individuals at risk for psychosis.57 The neuropsychological impairments in at-risk individuals tend to be associated with poor social and role functioning, and may predict the development of later psychosis.68

Despite the intuitive significance of deficits in social functioning and their importance in prodromal research, there is a dearth of longitudinal studies to explore social functioning at outcome. The present study is an attempt to elucidate the relationship between clinical symptomatology and neurocognition at initial presentation, and social functioning 1 year after ascertainment in subjects at risk for psychosis.

METHODS

Participants

The Cognitive Assessment and Risk Evaluation (CARE) program at the University of California, San Diego, provides longitudinal assessment of individuals who are considered to be at risk for developing psychosis.9 For this study, we selected a subsample (N=22) of at-risk subjects from previously published reports6,10,11 who had received the Social Adjustment Scale-Self-Report (SAS-SR) at 1-year follow-up.

Procedure

All participants received a comprehensive clinical and neurocognitive battery at initial assessment.6 The clinical assessment included the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) and the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes (SIPS).12

The SAS-SR was administered to all participants at baseline assessment and at 1-year follow-up. Domains related to work/school role and social/leisure time were examined. We did not examine the two SAS-SR family domains (family unit and family outside of the home) because of the young age of our sample.

The neurocognitive battery6,11 included the Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test (WCST), the Stroop Color–Word Test, the Numerical Attention test, the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test (HVLT), Revised, the Spatial Span subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition, as well as the Letter Number Sequencing, Block Design, and Vocabulary subtests of the WAIS, 3rd Edition.

Statistical Analyses

Analyses were performed with SPSS. Change in symptom severity and social functioning over time was assessed via paired-samples t-tests. We performed correlations between follow-up social functioning measures and baseline clinical and neurocognitive batteries to determine which variables should be included in regression analyses (with significance at p<0.05). Backward multiple-regression analyses were then performed using baseline social functioning, clinical, and neurocognitive variables as predictors of follow-up social functioning. The assumptions of normality, linearity, and homoscedasticity of residuals were met. With the use of a p<0.001 criterion for Mahalanobis distance, no outliers among the cases were found. No cases had missing data, and no suppressor variables were found (N=22).

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

The baseline demographic characteristics and neurocognitive scores of our sample are presented in Table 1. The rate of transition to psychosis in our sample was 9.1%; two subjects converted to schizophrenia over the 1-year follow-up.

TABLE 1. Demographic, Clinical, and Neurocognitive Characteristics of the At-Risk Sample at Baseline and Follow-Up (N=22)

SD: standard deviation; WCST: Wisconsin Card-Sorting Test; SCWT: Stroop Color and Word Test; HVLT–R: Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised; WAIS-III: WechslerAdult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edition; WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd Edition; SIPS: Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes; GAF: Global Assessment of Functioning; SAS: Social Adjustment Scale.

TABLE 1. Demographic, Clinical, and Neurocognitive Characteristics of the At-Risk Sample at Baseline and Follow-Up (N=22)
Enlarge table

Change in Clinical Symptomatology and Level of Social Functioning Over Time

As shown in Table 1, paired-samples t-tests showed significant improvement between baseline and follow-up on the Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) and all of the SIPS ratings. However, there were no significant changes over time on any of the SAS-SR ratings. The at-risk subjects remained impaired in their social functioning at follow-up.

Prediction of Social-Functioning Outcomes

Given our relatively small sample size, we limited the number of predictor variables that were used in the regression analyses to those that had significant correlations (p<0.05) with the social functioning variables at follow-up (r=0.45 to 0.67). Yet, for each dependent variable, the corresponding baseline variable was entered into the prediction model regardless of the magnitude of the correlation between them. Interestingly, the outcome social-functioning measures were not significantly correlated with baseline GAF, SIPS positive symptoms, or performance on the WCST. In order to avoid multicollinearity problems, we examined the correlations among the predictor variables. Those correlations were all small-to-moderate (r=0.43 to 0.59; p<0.05) except for the high association between SIPS Disorganized and SIPS Negative (r=0.71; p<0.001).

We decided to include both of those variables in the regression analyses because they represent different symptom domains. We performed three backward multiple-regression analyses, one for each SAS outcome variable.

When the four variables that were significantly correlated with follow-up, Overall SAS, in addition to baseline Overall SAS scores, were included in the first backward multiple regression; we observed a significant regression coefficient that accounted for 70% of the variance in overall functioning at outcome: F[5, 15]=7.18; p=0.001; R2=0.70 (at Step 1). After Stroop Color Naming and SIPS Negative were excluded from the model, baseline Overall SAS, SIPS Disorganized, and Stroop Color/Word Interference still accounted for 70% of the variance in follow-up Overall SAS: F[3, 17]=13.29; p<0.001; R2=0.70 (at Step 3). The most significant predictor of overall functioning was SIPS Disorganized (β=0.59), followed by Stroop Color/Word Interference (β = –0.44). Baseline SAS Overall was not a significant predictor (β=0.28; p=0.06).

When the four variables that were significantly correlated with follow-up SAS Social/Leisure, in addition to baseline SAS Social/Leisure, were included in the second backward multiple regression, we observed a significant regression coefficient that accounted for 64% of the variance in social role functioning at outcome: F[5, 15]=5.42; p=0.005; R2=0.64 (at Step 1). After SAS Social/Leisure and SIPS Negative were excluded from the model, the three-predictor model, including SIPS Disorganized, HVLT Total Recall, and Stroop Color/Word Interference, accounted for 61% of the variance in follow-up SAS Social/Leisure, F[3, 17]=8.78; p=0.001; R2=0.61 (at Step 3). The most significant predictor of social role functioning was SIPS Disorganized (β=0.49), followed by the Stroop Color/Word Interference (β = –0.37). HVLT's regression coefficient was not significant (β = –0.31; p=0.08).

When the three variables that were significantly correlated with follow-up SAS Work Role, in addition to baseline SAS Work Role, were included in the third backward multiple regression, we observed a significant regression coefficient that accounted for 81% of the variance in Work Role Functioning at outcome: F[4, 11]=11.62; p=0.001; R2=0.81 (at Step 1). After baseline SAS Work Role was excluded from the model, Stroop Color-Naming, Stroop Color/Word Interference, and Numerical Attention significantly contributed to the prediction of follow-up SAS Work Role, accounting for 79% of the variance: F[3, 12]=14.92; p<0.001; R2=0.79 (at Step 2). The most significant predictor of follow-up Work Role Functioning was Stroop Color-Naming (β = –0.50), followed by the Stroop Color/Word Interference (β = –0.38), and Numerical Attention (β=0.31).

DISCUSSION

Our results showed that disorganized symptoms and executive functioning deficits at baseline assessment were significant predictors of overall social functioning and the subscale of social role functioning. Work role impairment was also accounted for by deficits in executive functioning, as well as processing speed.

Impairment in social functioning is arguably the most debilitating aspect of schizophrenia, in the broader context of an individual's lifelong struggle with the disorder. Interestingly, our results indicate that longitudinal outcomes of social functioning impairment are independent of positive symptoms. This finding is noteworthy, as it suggests that regardless of successful treatment of positive symptoms, individuals at risk for schizophrenia continue to have social functioning deficits.13,14 Therefore, assessment of and early intervention for impairments in social functioning are unquestionably important in this population.

This finding, if corroborated by larger studies, suggests that interventions addressing social functioning in this vulnerable group are indicated. Social skills training, individualized educational programs, and family psychoeducation and/or psychotherapy could all help reduce stress and improve functional outcome. Also, psychosocial treatment efforts that focus on cognitive training or remediation might help to ameliorate the observed functional deficits in this vulnerable group.14,15

From the University of California, Los Angeles, Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology; and the University of California, San Diego, Department of Psychiatry.
Correspondence: Kristin S. Cadenhead, M.D.; (e-mail).

1. Davidson M , Reichenberg A , Rabinowitz J , et al.: Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1328–1335MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Keith SJ , Matthews SM : The diagnosis of schizophrenia: a review of onset and duration issues. Schizophr Bull 1991;17: 51–67Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. Yung AR , McGorry PD , McFarlane CA , et al.: Monitoring and care of young people at incipient risk of psychosis. Schizophr Bull 1996; 22: 283–303Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Cannon TD , Cadenhead KS , Cornblatt B , et al.: Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk: a multisite longitudinal study in North America. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:28–37Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Cosway R , Byrne M , Clafferty R , et al.: Neuropsychological change in young people at high risk for schizophrenia: results from the first two neuropsychological assessments of the Edinburgh High-Risk Study. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1111–1121Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Eastvold AD , Heaton RK , Cadenhead KS : Neurocognitive deficits in the (putative) prodrome and first episode of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2007; 93:266–277Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Niendam TA , Bearden CE , Johnson JK , et al.: Neurocognitive performance and functional disability in the psychosis prodrome. Schizophr Res 2006; 84:100–111Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. Niendam TA , Bearden CE , Zinberg J , et al.: The course of neurocognition and social functioning in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:772–781Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Seeber K , Cadenhead KS : How does studying schizotypal personality disorder inform us about the prodrome of schizophrenia? Curr Psychiatry Reports 2005; 7:41–50Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Ballon JS , Kaur T , Marks II , et al.: Social functioning in young people at risk for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2007; 151:29–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Jahshan C , Heaton RK , Golshan S , et al.: Course of neurocognitive deficits in the prodrome and first episode of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 2010; 24:109–120Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Miller TJ , McGlashan TH , Rosen JL , et al.: Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:703–715Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Cornblatt BA , Lencz T , Smith CW , et al.: The schizophrenia prodrome revisited: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29:633–651Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14. Haroun N , Dunn L , Haroun A , et al.: Risk and protection in prodromal schizophrenia: ethical implications for clinical practice and future research. Schizophr Bull 2003; 32:166–178CrossrefGoogle Scholar

15. Addington J , Addington D : Patterns of premorbid functioning in first-episode psychosis: relationship to 2-year outcome. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005; 112:40–46Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar