The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Objective:

Deep brain stimulation (DBS) is effective for the motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease (PD). Although most patients benefit with minimal cognitive side effects, cognitive decline is a risk, and there is little available evidence to guide preoperative risk assessment. Visual illusions or visual hallucinations (VHs) and impulse-control behaviors (ICBs) are relatively common complications of PD and its treatment and may be a marker of more advanced disease, but their relationship with postoperative cognition has not been established. The authors aimed to determine whether any preoperative history of VHs or ICBs is associated with cognitive change after DBS.

Methods:

Retrospective chart review identified 54 patients with PD who received DBS of the subthalamic nucleus or globus pallidus internus and who completed both pre- and postoperative neuropsychological testing. Linear regression models were used to assess whether any preoperative history of VHs or ICBs was associated with changes in attention, executive function, language, memory, or visuospatial cognitive domains while controlling for surgical target and duration between evaluations.

Results:

The investigators found that a history of VHs was associated with declines in attention (b=−4.04, p=0.041) and executive function (b=−4.24, p=0.021). A history of ICBs was not associated with any significant changes.

Conclusions:

These results suggest that a history of VHs may increase risk of cognitive decline after DBS; thus, specific preoperative counseling and targeted remediation strategies for these patients may be indicated. In contrast, a history of ICBs does not appear to be associated with increased cognitive risk.

Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) (STN-DBS) or globus pallidus internus (GPi) can significantly improve the motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease (PD) (1, 2). One concern with the neurosurgical procedure and chronic DBS, however, has been the risk of hastening the cognitive decline frequently associated with PD (36). Currently, there is no consensus on which presurgical characteristics elevate the risk of postoperative cognitive decline after DBS. Identifying these risk factors would inform a more personalized approach to care; in particular, about whether to proceed with surgery and, if so, how to help select the safest surgical approach, target, and stimulation strategy (7, 8).

Visual illusions or visual hallucinations (VHs) and impulse-control behaviors (ICBs) are two common nonmotor symptoms of PD, particularly after treatment with dopaminergic medications. In our clinic, we anecdotally observed that patients who reported a history of VHs experienced worse cognitive outcomes postoperatively, whereas those with a history of ICBs seemed to remain cognitively stable. Although VHs and ICBs are distinct phenomena, both may reflect unique neural network abnormalities or vulnerabilities and, as such, may provide prognostic information about cognitive outcomes after DBS.

In the case of VHs, prevalence in PD has been estimated at 22%-38% (912), and they occur on a spectrum from minor illusions to vivid hallucinations (13, 14). Although their etiology is likely complex (1517), occurrence and progression in severity may be a behavioral marker of a more advanced and widespread neuronal pathology (18) and have been associated with cognitive changes (1922) and reduced quality of life (13, 23, 24). This raises important questions about potential cognitive risks of DBS in patients with a history of VHs. Two studies incidentally described that psychosis-like symptoms in the immediate preoperative period appear to be associated with development of postoperative dementia (25, 26). To our knowledge, no study has systematically explored whether any history of VHs is associated with increased risk of post-DBS cognitive decline.

Similar to VHs, ICBs are fairly prevalent (in up to 20% of patients with PD), albeit associated primarily with dopaminergic therapy (27, 28). Unlike VHs, however, ICBs generally have not been linked to cognitive decline, although there have been reports suggesting differences in executive function between patients with and those without ICBs (29, 30). One study investigated relationships between ICBs and STN-DBS and found no cognitive differences at baseline or postoperatively between patients with and those without ICBs (31).

Motivated by our anecdotal clinical observations and to help clarify whether these common behavioral symptoms of PD and its treatment may reflect vulnerability to post-DBS cognitive decline, we used a retrospective clinical cohort to assess whether any preoperative history of VHs or ICBs was associated with clinically meaningful postoperative cognitive changes. As a secondary analysis, we determined whether cognitive outcomes in such patients differed depending on targeting of STN versus GPi.

Methods

Participants

We retrospectively reviewed electronic medical records of all patients with a diagnosis of PD who underwent DBS implantation in the STN or GPi between December 2012 and March 2018 (N=172), in accordance with a protocol approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. Additional inclusion criteria included bilateral surgery, ongoing bilateral active stimulation after activation of the device, and the presence of both pre- and postoperative neuropsychological testing data. Exclusion criteria were clinical diagnosis of dementia at the time of preoperative evaluation, a Mini-Mental Status Examination score <24 at preoperative neuropsychological testing, and explant of the DBS device before postoperative neuropsychological testing. Lead implantation was performed as previously described utilizing either microelectrode recording or intraoperative MRI (32).

Chart Review

Demographic and relevant clinical data were collected for each subject by an abstractor (I.H.K.) who was blind to neuropsychological testing results. Medications at the time of both pre- and postoperative neuropsychological testing were reconciled, and levodopa equivalent daily dosages (33) were calculated for medications prescribed for motor symptoms. The preoperative number of nonmotor medications and Charlson Comorbidity Index score (34) were used as measures of medical comorbidity. Neurology and neurosurgery clinical notes were reviewed to confirm that each patient was receiving bilateral stimulation throughout the postoperative course, including during postoperative neuropsychological testing.

History of VHs was defined as documentation of any history of a visual misperception or hallucination outside the setting of acute delirium before DBS. History of ICBs was defined as documentation of a notable increase from baseline in impulsive or repetitive behaviors, typically related to shopping, sexual drive, or gambling. In addition to reviewing relevant medical and neuropsychological notes, we also utilized the Epic Systems, (Verona, WI) search function for the terms “hallucination,” “VH,” “illusion,” “shadow,” “impulse,” “compulsive,” “ICD,” “shopping,” and “gambling” to reduce the likelihood of missing the documentation of any such occurrences.

Neuropsychological Testing

All patients but one were assessed in our clinic. Before analyzing the data, we decided to utilize only a subset of tests from the full batteries with the goal of assessing all major cognitive domains but minimizing the number of statistical comparisons to decrease the risk of type I errors. On the basis of previous reports (3537), we ultimately included 15 different measures: the Trail-Making Test, Part B; the Color-Word Interference Test (Stroop analog from the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System); the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence–II Matrix Reasoning and Similarities subtests; the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Semantic Fluency test, List Learning test, List Recall test, Figure Copy test, Figure Recall test, and Digit Span Forward test; the Boston Naming Test; the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading; the Controlled Oral-Word Association test; and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Digit Span Backward and Picture Completion subtests. These measures were ultimately consolidated into five cognitive domains (Table 1) to reduce the number of statistical comparisons and provide more clinically relevant results. Some patients were unable to complete all measures for various reasons (e.g., fatigue, sensoriperceptual problem), but all patients completed at least 13 of the measures both pre- and postoperatively, with the exception of two patients preoperatively and three patients postoperatively. One patient’s scores for the Digit Span Forward and Digit Span Backward subtests were excluded because of poor performance that was judged at the time of assessment by our neuropsychologist (L.C.H.) to reflect hearing impairment and not cognitive ability. Raw scores were converted to psychometric T scores (mean=50, standard deviation=10) to control for normative variables (i.e., age, sex, and level of education) and to facilitate direct comparison between pre- and postoperative scores.

TABLE 1. Effects of the surgical target and duration between neuropsychological testing on postoperative change in neuropsychological testinga

Cognitive domain and testSurgical target (subthalamic nucleus)Duration between neuropsychological testing
BbSEcpBbSEcp
Attention+1.592.150.462–0.880.800.274
 RBANS Digit Span Forward
 WAIS Digit Span Backward
 WAIS Picture Completion
Executive function–0.611.940.754–2.220.730.004*
 Color-Word Interference Test (Stroop analog)
 Controlled Oral-Word Association Test
 Trail-Making Test, Part B
 WAIS Digit Span Backward
 WASI-II Matrix Reasoning
 WASI-II Similarities
Language+1.421.300.281–0.930.490.063
 Boston Naming Test
 Controlled Oral-Word Association Test
 RBANS Semantic Fluency
 WASI-II Similarities
 Wechsler Test of Adult Reading
Memory–1.692.680.532–3.120.950.002*
 RBANS List Learning
 RBANS List Recall
 RBANS Figure Recall
Visuospatial–0.582.110.785–1.250.750.103
 Boston Naming Test
 RBANS Figure Copy
 RBANS Figure Recall
 WAIS Picture Completion
 WASI-II Matrix Reasoning

aRBANS=Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status, WAIS=Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, WASI-II=Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence–II.

bData represent linear regression coefficients.

cData represent standard error of the means.

*p<0.05.

TABLE 1. Effects of the surgical target and duration between neuropsychological testing on postoperative change in neuropsychological testinga

Enlarge table

Statistical Analysis

The distributions of continuous variables were checked for normality, and they were compared with paired or two-sample t tests, as appropriate, or Welch’s test for data with unequal variances. The chi-square test was used to compare categorical variables. For direct statistical comparison between descriptors of VH and ICB groups only, three patients who had a history of both symptoms were excluded. Mean preoperative and postoperative T scores were estimated for each subject individually and by cognitive domain according to the respective items in that domain (Table 1). To quantify changes in each domain, we then subtracted the mean preoperative T score from the mean postoperative T score to generate a ΔT score. Finally, associations between neuropsychiatric variable (VHs or ICBs) and cognitive domain were evaluated using linear regression. Covariates for the regression models initially included duration between neuropsychological testing and surgical target, and preoperative cognition-by-domain was added as a third covariate as a secondary analysis (i.e., preoperative mean T score for executive function was included as a covariate for the analysis of postoperative change in executive function, and so forth for each of the five cognitive domains). Significance level was set as α=0.05. The analytic plan to use linear regression models with covariates to control for surgical target and duration between neuropsychological testing was determined a priori, and analyses were performed with SAS, version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.).

Results

Cohort Description

Chart review identified 54 patients out of a total operative sample of 172 (31%) who met entry criteria. From these 54 patients, we identified 15 (28%) with documented preoperative VHs (with illusions, N=2; hallucinations, N=13) and 12 (22%) with documented preoperative ICBs. These numbers include three (6%) patients with a documented history of both VHs and ICBs. The cohort overall was fairly homogeneous, consisting predominantly of right-handed, middle-aged, well-educated, Caucasian males who had been diagnosed with PD just under a decade before surgery (Table 2). Medical comorbidity was otherwise low. Mean time until follow-up neuropsychological testing was 1.4 years (median=0.77 years) with some variability.

TABLE 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients who received deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) or globus pallidus internusa

CharacteristicVisual hallucinationsImpulse-control behaviorsWhole cohort
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)68.39.062.85.966.57.7
N%N%N%
Male12808674176
Right-handedness149311924789
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Education (years)14.92.415.72.614.72.9
Duration of disease (years) 8.94.011.06.18.94.9
Number of preoperative PD nonmotor medications3.72.54.22.34.23.0
Charlson Comorbidity Index scoreb2.71.12.10.92.51.1
Number of preoperative PD motor medications2.71.33.41.22.81.2
Preoperative levodopa equivalent daily dosage (mg/day)1,1835021,4476871,094597
UPDRS-III score
 Preoperative off scorec41.113.537.513.441.013.0
 Preoperative off-on score change (% change)47.813.454.413.446.215.9
N%N%N%
Taking preoperative antidepressants640541935
STN target1173650%4278
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Duration between neuropsychological testing (years)1.71.51.61.21.41.2
Postoperative levodopa equivalent daily dosages (% change)31.639.546.325.032.040.7

aDifferences do not meet statistical significance unless otherwise noted. UPDRS-III=Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, part III.

bPredicts 10-year survival in patients with multiple comorbidities with increasing values associated with decreased survival. (For example, 2 points is associated with predicted 90% likelihood of survival over the next 10 years.)

cRange=0–56; increasing values represent worsened PD motor symptoms.

Significant difference (p<0.05) between impulse-control behavior yes-no conditions by two-sample t test.

Significant difference (p<0.05) between impulse-control behavior yes-no conditions by chi-square analysis.

TABLE 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients who received deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) or globus pallidus internusa

Enlarge table

Patients with a history of VHs or ICBs were similar at preoperative baseline to those without such a history (Table 2), with the following exceptions: Patients with a history of ICBs strongly trended toward being younger (62.8 years versus 67.6 years, p=0.053); were taking more PD medications for their motor symptoms (3.4 versus 2.6, p=0.045), with associated higher total levodopa equivalent daily dosages (1,447 mg versus 993 mg, p=0.019); had a greater improvement in preoperative Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, part III (UPDRS-III), scores in the medicated (on) versus unmedicated (off) condition (54.4% versus 43.8%, p=0.042); and were less likely to have had STN as their DBS target (50% versus 86%, p=0.016).

Baseline Cognition

At preoperative neuropsychological testing assessment, on average, patients with a history of VH or ICB performed indistinguishably in each cognitive domain. (A table [Table S1] describing patient data is available as an online supplement to this article.) When analyzed by surgical target, baseline testing revealed worse performance by the GPi subcohort as compared with the STN subcohort on measures of executive function (40.87 versus 49.99, p=0.0001), memory (38.95 versus 46.20, p=0.011), and visuospatial cognition (40.06 versus 49.43, p=0.0002) (see Table S2 in the online supplement).

Analysis and Linear Regression Models

Changes in cognition in entire sample.

Analysis of the entire cohort’s change between pre- and postoperative neuropsychological testing without controlling for covariates demonstrated significantly worsened performance in executive function (ΔT=−3.64, p=0.0001) and language measures (ΔT=−2.05, p=0.001) (see Table S3 in the online supplement).

Effects of target and duration between follow-up on cognitive decline.

We used linear regression models to determine whether duration between pre- and postoperative neuropsychological testing or surgical target significantly contributed to ΔT scores for each cognitive domain. We found that duration between neuropsychological testing, but not DBS target, significantly contributed to changes in cognitive performances (Table 1). Specifically, increased duration between neuropsychological testing was associated with mild worsening in executive function (b=−2.22, p=0.004) and memory (b=−3.12, p=0.002).

Effects of history of VHs or ICBs.

Next, we sought to determine whether a history of VHs or ICBs was associated with specific cognitive changes at follow-up neuropsychological testing. We again used linear regression models and added a term for neuropsychiatric history as an additional covariate, along with duration between neuropsychological testing and surgical target. As shown in Table 3, history of VHs explained approximately one-half of a standard deviation decrement in performance in attention (b=−4.04, p=0.041) and executive function (b=−4.24, p=0.021). Conversely, history of ICBs was not associated with any statistically significant post-DBS cognitive changes.

TABLE 3. Linear regression models of postoperative cognitive changes as a function of history of visual hallucinations (VHs) or impulse-control behaviors (ICBs)

VHsICBs
Cognitive domainBaSEbpBSEp
Attention−4.041.920.041*3.322.220.142
Executive function−4.241.770.021*3.142.070.135
Language0.211.260.870−0.051.420.970
Memory−2.882.480.252−3.672.940.219
Visuospatial−0.641.990.750+0.112.360.963

aData represent linear regression coefficients.

bData represent standard error of the means.

*p<0.05.

TABLE 3. Linear regression models of postoperative cognitive changes as a function of history of visual hallucinations (VHs) or impulse-control behaviors (ICBs)

Enlarge table

To determine whether the identified associations were driven by those patients with frank VHs, we excluded the two patients with visual illusions and repeated the analysis (Table S4 in the online supplement). The significant association between history of VHs and worsened executive function was preserved (b=−4.79, p=0.013), whereas the association with worsened attention became nonsignificant.

Next, we included preoperative cognition as an additional covariate in our models to determine its influence on outcomes. The results were largely unchanged (see Table S5 in the online supplement; for comparisons including or excluding patients with visual illusions, see Table 3 and Tables S4 and Table S5 in the online supplement), although the association between impaired postoperative attention and history of VHs became only borderline significant (p=0.050).

Finally, to further explore the impairments in attention and executive function associated with a history of VHs, separate linear regression models were carried out for each of the eight cognitive tests that were used to define the domains (Table 1). In contrast to the domain-based analysis, a history of VHs was not significantly associated with worsened performance on any individual measure (Table S6 in the online supplement).

Discussion

Using data from a retrospective surgical cohort at an academic medical center, we demonstrated that any preoperative history of VH was significantly associated with worsening of postoperative cognitive performance in attention and executive function in patients with PD receiving DBS. We did not find any such association with a preoperative history of ICBs. Surgical target (STN or GPi) did not affect cognitive outcomes, but increased duration between pre- and postoperative neuropsychological testing was consistently associated with mildly worsened performances on attention and memory tasks.

The notion that neuropsychiatric history might influence DBS outcomes has support in the literature. For example, one study found that patients with active preoperative diagnoses of mood or anxiety disorders had greater improvements in mood after DBS (38). A unique aspect of our approach, however, was to include any history of VH or ICB, regardless of whether the symptoms were present at the time of DBS surgery. Previous studies of these nonmotor symptoms generally categorized the presence of symptoms only if they occurred during the immediate presurgical period (e.g., 1 week previously for the UPDRS, section I, and 1 month previously for The Neuropsychiatric Inventory Questionnaire). For example, Funkiewiez et al. (39) utilized the thought disorder question in UPDRS section I (question 2) and identified only three of 70 patients (4%) preoperatively who had experienced hallucinations (or delusions) in the week before surgery. We reasoned that susceptibility to these symptoms, even if caused by dopaminergic medications and resolved after cessation of the causative agents, may imply underlying vulnerabilities in specific brain networks that could portend vulnerabilities for cognitive changes after anesthesia, neurosurgery, chronic brain stimulation, and associated medication changes.

This approach allowed us to identify 15 out of 54 patients (28%) who had ever experienced preoperative VHs and may explain why we were able to detect robust changes in cognition associated with a history of VHs that had been only hinted at in previous reports. Aybek and colleagues (25), for instance, found that all five (9%) of their initial 57 patients who had endorsed psychosis-like symptoms on their preoperative UPDRS, part I (item 2, score ≥2), were diagnosed with dementia 3 years later. Similarly, Perriol et al. (26) noted in their study of 58 patients that three out of the four who experienced postoperative cognitive complications had a “history of dopaminergic psychosis,” but this was not further characterized or discussed. Thus, although the use of retrospective chart review and lack of validated instruments to assess for history of VHs (and ICBs) may be considered a limitation, our approach may have identified patients with such a phenotype with greater sensitivity, thereby providing greater statistical power to identify cognitive declines associated with a history of VHs that only had been suggested previously.

Although this finding is concerning and, if replicated, might inform preoperative counseling, risk assessment, improved patient selection, and perhaps even remediation strategies, it is also important to stress that, on average, performance worsened only by less than one-half of one standard deviation in two of the five cognitive domains assessed. Depending on an individual patient’s burden of motor symptoms and overall clinical context, such outcomes may be considered an acceptable tradeoff, and a history of VHs alone does not appear to represent an absolute contraindication to DBS.

One potential concern with our approach was the grouping of patients with a history of illusions together with patients with a history of frank VHs, as considerably less is known about the prognostic implications of milder visual phenomenon (13). The small number of patients with a history of only illusions precluded analysis of these patients independently. Instead, we performed a sensitivity analysis by removing these patients and rerunning our linear regression models only with those patients with a documented history of frank VHs. We were surprised that removing patients with illusions eliminated the association between VHs and postoperative decline in attention (Tables S4 and S5 in the online supplement). This sensitivity analysis suggests that the variance is not explained solely by those patients with frank VHs and is consistent with other studies, indicating that even more minor visual phenomena may have clinical and prognostic significance (13, 23).

Another potential limitation was the possibility that differences in baseline cognition between the groups may explain the detected associations. At preoperative testing, however, the cognitive performance across groups was statistically indistinguishable (Table S1 in the online supplement). To further test the rigor of our approach, we repeated the analyses and included preoperative cognition-by-domain as an additional covariate. This sensitivity analysis did not change the outcome, reinforcing that differences in baseline cognitive performance did not explain our observations.

In the case of history of ICBs, we did not detect a significant post-DBS change in cognition, consistent with a previous report (31). This is compatible with the idea that ICBs may be attributable in part to iatrogenic overstimulation of the mesocortical and mesolimbic pathways as a consequence of pharmacological attempts to sufficiently augment the nigrostriatal (motor) system, which is then relieved by post-DBS dopaminergic medication reductions. Indeed, in our cohort, a history of ICBs was associated with both a greater preoperative burden of dopaminergic medication and a greater postoperative reduction (Table 2).

We also wondered whether surgical target might differentially influence results based on neuropsychiatric history, particularly given ongoing debate about putative cognitive differences associated with STN or GPi targeting (5, 40). We found no support for this idea. However, a major caveat is the nonrandom fashion in which surgical target was determined for each patient. To better understand what bias this might introduce, we subsequently reviewed the documented rationale for GPi targeting in each case. In eight of 12 cases (three of six ICB patients, five of six non-ICB patients), GPi was chosen because of identified preoperative concern about baseline cognition. Thus, if poorer baseline performance implied increased vulnerability and greater risk for further deterioration, then we might have found GPi stimulation to be associated with worsened results in our linear regression model. The fact that cognitive worsening was not explained by the choice of GPi as target could be interpreted as indirect evidence that GPi may be relatively cognitive sparing. Conversely, poorer preoperative performance in some cognitive domains in the GPi subcohort might have resulted in a floor effect that obscured our ability to detect more significant changes between targets. The smaller size of our GPi subcohort, however, combined with our methodological design, ultimately limits any conclusions.

We acknowledge several additional limitations. Although VHs are the most common form of psychotic symptoms in PD, we did not attempt to identify whether patients had delusions or other forms or hallucinations. Furthermore, there may have been unknown bias related to capturing only the patients who returned for follow-up neuropsychological testing, and the homogeneity of the demographics of the cohort and the academic setting may limit generalizability of our results to other populations. Another potential limitation is our lack of outcome data regarding motor symptoms and functional outcomes. As a retrospective study of a clinical population, we found that most patients did not have a documented postoperative UPDRS assessment to quantify motor or functional outcomes near the time of follow-up neuropsychological testing, if at all. As such, we could not adequately include this variable in our analysis, although we do note that postoperative decreases in dopaminergic medications—usually attributable to DBS-related improvement in motor symptoms—were not statistically different among our groups (Table 2), and past reports have not found a correlation between motor and cognitive outcomes (26). Nevertheless, we cannot exclude the possibility that motor or functional outcomes may have been different among the groups. Finally, and as noted earlier, identification of a history of VH or ICB was not based on any validated assessments. It is possible that our chart review failed to identify all patients who had ever experienced these symptoms or inaccurately identified patients who did not. Either possibility would result in a diluting of statistical power, and so the fact that we were able to identify significant effects further reinforces the robustness of our findings.

In conclusion, any history of VH during the course of PD may convey prognostic information about post-DBS cognitive risk. Further research is needed to determine whether these findings can be replicated in a larger, more diverse cohort for which motor and functional outcomes (i.e., scores on the UPDRS parts I–IV) are also available for inclusion in the analysis. With few, if any, validated predictors of DBS outcomes beyond preoperative response to levodopa, routine expansion of pre-DBS assessment to a more comprehensive survey of potential neuropsychiatric symptoms may be an important step toward optimizing DBS-related decision making for each individual patient.

Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh (Kratter, Karp); Department of Neurological Surgery, Brain Modulation Laboratory, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine (Kratter, Chang, Whiteman, Feyder, Jorge, Henry); Department of Neurosurgery, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston (Richardson); University of Arizona College of Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, Tucson (Karp); and Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, Calif. (Kratter).
Send correspondence to Dr. Kratter ().

Previously presented in part as a poster at the annual meeting of the American Neuropsychiatric Association, March 20–23, 2019, Chicago.

Supported by the Hamot Health Foundation and the University of Pittsburgh Department of Psychiatry internal funding.

Dr. Karp has received medication supplies from Indivior and Pfizer for investigator-initiated trials; he has received compensation from Otsuka for the development and presentation of an educational webinar (disease-focused, not product specific), from the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry and the American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry for editorial work, and from NightWare for scientific advising; and he serves on the advisory board of Aifred Health. Dr. Richardson serves as a consultant to Boston Scientific and Medtronic. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

The authors thank the University of Pittsburgh Department of Psychiatry Office of Residency Training for providing dedicated research time to allow for this research. The authors also thank Dr. Amber van Laar, Danielle Corson, PA-C, and members of the Richardson laboratory for helpful conversations and feedback.

References

1 Okun MS : Deep-brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease . N Engl J Med 2012 ; 367 : 1529 – 1538 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Almeida L , Deeb W , Spears C , et al. : Current practice and the future of deep brain stimulation therapy in Parkinson’s disease . Semin Neurol 2017 ; 37 : 205 – 214 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Hely MA , Reid WGJ , Adena MA , et al. : The Sydney Multicenter Study of Parkinson’s Disease: the inevitability of dementia at 20 years . Mov Disord 2008 ; 23 : 837 – 844 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Aarsland D , Kurz MW : The epidemiology of dementia associated with Parkinson disease . J Neurol Sci 2010 ; 289 : 18 – 22 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Rothlind JC , York MK , Carlson K , et al. : Neuropsychological changes following deep brain stimulation surgery for Parkinson’s disease: comparisons of treatment at pallidal and subthalamic targets versus best medical therapy . J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2015 ; 86 : 622 – 629 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Hogg E , Wertheimer J , Graner S , et al. : Deep brain stimulation and nonmotor symptoms . Int Rev Neurobiol 2017 ; 134 : 1045 – 1089 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Lang AE , Houeto JL , Krack P , et al. : Deep brain stimulation: preoperative issues . Mov Disord 2006 ; 21 ( Suppl 14 ): S171 – S196 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Bronstein JM , Tagliati M , Alterman RL , et al. : Deep brain stimulation for Parkinson disease: an expert consensus and review of key issues . Arch Neurol 2011 ; 68 : 165 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Fénelon G , Alves G : Epidemiology of psychosis in Parkinson’s disease . J Neurol Sci 2010 ; 289 : 12 – 17 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Ffytche DH , Creese B , Politis M , et al. : The psychosis spectrum in Parkinson disease . Nat Rev Neurol 2017 ; 13 : 81 – 95 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Diederich NJ , Fénelon G , Stebbins G , et al. : Hallucinations in Parkinson disease . Nat Rev Neurol 2009 ; 5 : 331 – 342 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Trojano L , Papagno C : Cognitive and behavioral disorders in Parkinson’s disease: an update. II: behavioral disorders . Neurol Sci 2018 ; 39 : 53 – 61 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Lenka A , Pagonabarraga J , Pal PK , et al. : Minor hallucinations in Parkinson disease: a subtle symptom with major clinical implications . Neurology 2019 ; 93 : 259 – 266 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Goetz CG , Fan W , Leurgans S , et al. : The malignant course of “benign hallucinations” in Parkinson disease . Arch Neurol 2006 ; 63 : 713 – 716 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Factor SA , McDonald WM , Goldstein FC : The role of neurotransmitters in the development of Parkinson’s disease-related psychosis . Eur J Neurol 2017 ; 24 : 1244 – 1254 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Goldman JG , Stebbins GT , Dinh V , et al. : Visuoperceptive region atrophy independent of cognitive status in patients with Parkinson’s disease with hallucinations . Brain 2014 ; 137 : 849 – 859 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Goetz CG , Vaughan CL , Goldman JG , et al. : I finally see what you see: Parkinson’s disease visual hallucinations captured with functional neuroimaging . Mov Disord 2014 ; 29 : 115 – 117 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Kempster PA , O’Sullivan SS , Holton JL , et al. : Relationships between age and late progression of Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study . Brain 2010 ; 133 : 1755 – 1762 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Bernard BA , Carns D , Stebbins GT , et al. : Relationship of Movement Disorders Society-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale nonmotor symptoms to cognitive functioning in patients with Parkinson’s disease . Mov Disord Clin Pract (Hoboken) 2020 ; 7 : 279 – 283 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Lenka A , Hegde S , Arumugham SS , et al. : Pattern of cognitive impairment in patients with Parkinson’s disease and psychosis: a critical review . Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2017 ; 37 : 11 – 18 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Szeto JY , O’Callaghan C , Shine JM , et al. : The relationships between mild cognitive impairment and phenotype in Parkinson’s disease . NPJ Parkinsons Dis 2015 ; 1 : 15015 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Barnes J , Boubert L : Executive functions are impaired in patients with Parkinson’s disease with visual hallucinations . J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008 ; 79 : 190 – 192 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Mack J , Rabins P , Anderson K , et al. : Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in a community-based Parkinson disease sample . Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2012 ; 20 : 123 – 132 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Fénelon G , Mahieux F , Huon R , et al. : Hallucinations in Parkinson’s disease: prevalence, phenomenology and risk factors . Brain 2000 ; 123 : 733 – 745 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Aybek S , Gronchi-Perrin A , Berney A , et al. : Long-term cognitive profile and incidence of dementia after STN-DBS in Parkinson’s disease . Mov Disord 2007 ; 22 : 974 – 981 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Perriol MP , Krystkowiak P , Defebvre L , et al. : Stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson’s disease: cognitive and affective changes are not linked to the motor outcome . Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2006 ; 12 : 205 – 210 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Voon V , Mehta AR , Hallett M : Impulse control disorders in Parkinsonʼs disease: recent advances . Curr Opin Neurol 2011 ; 24 : 324 – 330 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Weintraub D , Claassen DO : Impulse control and related disorders in Parkinson’s disease . Int Rev Neurobiol 2017 ; 133 : 679 – 717 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Vitale C , Santangelo G , Trojano L , et al. : Comparative neuropsychological profile of pathological gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive eating in Parkinson’s disease . Mov Disord 2011 ; 26 : 830 – 836 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Santangelo G , Vitale C , Trojano L , et al. : Cognitive dysfunctions and pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson’s disease . Mov Disord 2009 ; 24 : 899 – 905 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Merola A , Romagnolo A , Rizzi L , et al. : Impulse control behaviors and subthalamic deep brain stimulation in Parkinson disease . J Neurol 2017 ; 264 : 40 – 48 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Lee PS , Richardson RM : Interventional MRI-guided deep brain stimulation lead implantation . Neurosurg Clin N Am 2017 ; 28 : 535 – 544 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Tomlinson CL , Stowe R , Patel S , et al. : Systematic review of levodopa dose equivalency reporting in Parkinson’s disease . Mov Disord 2010 ; 25 : 2649 – 2653 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Charlson ME , Pompei P , Ales KL , et al. : A new method of classifying prognostic comorbidity in longitudinal studies: development and validation . J Chronic Dis 1987 ; 40 : 373 – 383 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Williams-Gray CH , Evans JR , Goris A , et al. : The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN cohort . Brain 2009 ; 132 : 2958 – 2969 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Foley JA , Foltynie T , Limousin P , et al. : Standardised neuropsychological assessment for the selection of patients undergoing DBS for Parkinson’s disease . Parkinsons Dis 2018 ; 2018 : 4328371 MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Tröster AI : Some clinically useful information that neuropsychology provides patients, carepartners, neurologists, and neurosurgeons about deep brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease . Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2017 ; 32 : 810 – 828 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Eisenstein SA , Dewispelaere WB , Campbell MC , et al. : Acute changes in mood induced by subthalamic deep brain stimulation in Parkinson disease are modulated by psychiatric diagnosis . Brain Stimul 2014 ; 7 : 701 – 708 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Funkiewiez A , Ardouin C , Caputo E , et al. : Long term effects of bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation on cognitive function, mood, and behaviour in Parkinson’s disease . J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004 ; 75 : 834 – 839 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Hansen AL , Krell-Roesch J , Kirlin KA , et al. : Deep brain stimulation and cognitive outcomes among patients with Parkinson’s disease: a historical cohort study . J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2019 ; 31 : 196 – 200 LinkGoogle Scholar