The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

Subcortical ischemic vascular disease (SIVD) is characterized by extensive white-matter lesions and lacunar infarcts in deep gray matter. The aim of this study was to investigate patterns of cognitive impairment in patients with SIVD. In a retrospective analysis, the authors compared the cognitive performance of 58 patients meeting MRI-defined criteria for SIVD (26 women; 47.3%) with age- and gender-matched control subjects. SIVD patients showed impairments in measures of verbal fluency, verbal memory, speed of cognitive processing, and divided attention. There were no significant differences in constructional praxis, figurative memory, verbal recognition memory, or semantic processing.

Vascular dementia is a heterogeneous group of diseases with a wide variety of etiological mechanisms, brain changes, and clinical manifestations.13 Cortical vascular dementia, strategic infarct dementia, and subcortical vascular dementia are the major subtypes of vascular dementia.4

Subcortical vascular dementia is defined by the accumulation of extensive cerebral white-matter lesions and lacunar infarcts in deep gray matter, whereby dementia is the clinical end-stage of a progressive disease.5 Strong evidence suggests the role of white-matter lesions as determinants of cognitive decline.6 Since there is growing evidence that white-matter lesions cause specific changes in neurocognitive performance in patients not meeting the diagnostic criteria for dementia, the focus in research is shifting from this late stage to the early stages of the disease. Furthermore, white-matter lesions are no longer considered an innocuous finding because they are associated, in cross-sectional surveys, with various disturbances and, in follow-up studies, with poor prognosis.7

On the basis of MRI criteria, Erkinjuntti8 introduced the concept of subcortical ischemic vascular disease (SIVD). SIVD arises from small-vessel disease and is characterized by extensive cerebral white-matter lesions and lacunar infarcts in deep gray and white matter.4 The multicentric Leukariosis and Disability Study (LADIS) described the clinical and cognitive characteristics of MRI-defined SIVD in a sample of elderly subjects. In the latter study, SIVD was associated with motor impairment, a history of falls, and subtle impairment in activities of daily living. As compared with controls, SIVD subjects performed significantly worse in tests measuring global cognitive functioning, psychomotor speed, attention, executive functions, verbal fluency, and working memory.3 In another study, SIVD was related to progressive cognitive impairment and a considerable risk of developing dementia.9 However, empirical data describing the cognitive impacts of SIVD are still sparse.

Studies investigating the impact of the accumulation of subcortical lesions on cognitive performance postulate that other confounding brain measures also affect cognition;10 these include global atrophy,11 central atrophy,12 medial temporal lobe atrophy,13 and frontal atrophy.10 Severe white-matter lesions predict cerebral atrophy,14 especially in the frontal and medial temporal lobe.15,16

In our retrospective analysis, we compared 58 patients meeting MRI-defined criteria for SIVD (26 women; 47.3%) with age- and gender-matched controls.

Methods

Subjects and Study Design

In our retrospective study-design, patients were identified who were admitted to the University Hospital of Salzburg, Department of Neurology, from June 1, 2007 to June 30, 2011. An electronic database selected patients who were free of unrelated psychiatric and neurological disease and had undergone a cerebral Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI) procedure with a minimum number of images and a neuropsychological examination (N=321). MRI and neuropsychological testing were performed within 3 months. Reasons for referral were planned carotid artery stenting because of asymptomatic carotid artery disease and subjective cognitive impairment. Patients’ records and results of brain MRI images were rechecked again manually (by M.S.) and the following exclusion criteria were applied: presence of severe psychiatric disease (e.g., depression, schizophrenia spectrum disorders), presence of severe unrelated central nervous disease (e.g., transient ischemic attack, stroke, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy) and leukoencephalopathy of non-vascular origin (e.g., immunological-demyelinating, metabolic, toxic, infectious).

SIVD patients were identified according to the brain-imaging criteria proposed by Erkinjuntti8 (N=58). The age- and gender-matched control group comprised 58 patients who did not meet the applied SIVD criteria.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)

MRI was performed on a 3-Tesla, whole-body MRI scanner (Achieva; Philips, the Netherlands). Coronar T1-weighted images (36 slices, TR: 451 msec, TE: 13 msec, thickness: 4 mm/1, flip: 80, Turbo Factor: 1, matrix: 512, time: 2:31 minutes); axial T2 images (28 slices, TR: 3,048 msec, TE: 80 msec, thickness: 4 mm/1, Turbo factor: 15, EPI factor: 1, matrix: 512, time: 1:19 minutes); and fluid attention inversion recovery (FLAIR) axial sequences (28 slices, TR: 10,000 msec, TE: 125 msec, TI: 2,800 msec, thickness: 4 mm/1, Turbo factor: 32, EPI factor: 1, matrix: 560, time: 3:30 minutes) were required.

Medial temporal lobe atrophy was graded by two raters (M.S. and Y.K.), blinded to neuropsychological data, on a 5-point Likert-scale17 considering the width of the choroid fissure, the width of the temporal horn, and the height of the hippocampal formation. Frontal lobe atrophy was rated by two raters (M.S. and Y.K.) blinded to neuropsychological data on a visual 4-point Likert scale (none, mild, moderate, severe) considering sulcal and ventricular sizes. In case of rater disagreement, mean scores of the two ratings were used for calculation.

Changes in cerebral white matter were rated by an experienced neurologist (M.S.) and an experienced neuroradiologist (M.M.), blinded to neuropsychological data, according to the revised version of the Fazekas scale18 and classified as “none” (0; no white-matter lesions), “mild” (1; single lesions <10 mm and grouped lesions <20 mm in any diameter), “moderate” (2; single lesions 10 mm–20 mm, grouped lesions >20 mm with connecting bridges only between the individual lesions), or “severe” (3; confluent lesions with >20 mm in any diameter). In case of rater disagreement, consensus was reached in a second rating session.

MRI-defined SIVD was diagnosed according to the brain-imaging criteria proposed by Erkinjuntti,8 which include 1) cases with predominantly white-matter lesions (WML), that is, extending periventricular and deep white-matter lesions (extending caps or irregular halo and diffusely confluent hyperintensities or extensive white-matter changes) and lacunas (at least 1); and 2) cases with predominantly lacunar infarcts, that is, >5 lacunas and at least moderate WML (extending caps or irregular halo or diffusely confluent hyperintensities or extensive white-matter changes). We also required absence of cortical and/or cortico–subcortical non-lacunar territorial infarcts, watershed infarcts, hemorrhages, signs of normal-pressure hydrocephalus, and specific causes of WML of non-vascular origin (e.g., multiple sclerosis, sarcoidosis, residual lesions of vasculitis).

In case of rater disagreement, consensus was reached in a second rating session.

Neuropsychological Examination

Each patient completed the Mini-Mental State Exam,19 and the neuropsychological test battery established by the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD-Plus).20,21 The CERAD is composed of several subtests: Semantic Verbal Fluency (SVF), Phonemic Verbal Fluency (PVF), Boston Naming Test (BNT), Word-List Learning (WLL), Word-List Delayed Recall (WLDR), Word-List Recognition (WLR), Discriminability (D), Figure Copy (FC), Figure Recall (FR), and Trail-Making Tests–A and B (TMT-A, B).

The test battery assesses verbal fluency (SVF, PVF), constructional praxis (FC), figurative memory (FR), verbal short-term and long-term memory (WLL, WLDR, WLR), verbal recognition memory (D), semantic processing (BNT), speed of cognitive processing (TMT A), and divided attention (TMT B).

All given values are z-scores adjusted for level of education, age, and gender.

Statistical Analysis

SPSS Student Version 16.0 was used for computation. A p level <0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

When comparing individuals with SIVD versus the non-SIVD reference group, the Mann-Whitney U test or chi-squared test were used as appropriate.

To delineate the association of white-matter lesion load with cognitive test scores, univariate analysis was used. Interobserver reliability is expressed by complete agreement in percent and kappa values.

Results

Demographic Characteristics

The 116 patients included had a median age of 79 (range: 44 to 89); 64 (52.7%) were men; 52 (47.3%) were women. The control group was well matched for age and gender (Table 1).

TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics and MRI Findings
No SIVD (N=58)SIVD (N=58)p
Demographic variables
 Age, years74.2 (11.4)74.5 (11.4)
 Women, N (%)26 (47.3)26 (47.3)
MRI findings
 White-matter lesions (Fazekas Scale)18
 None40
 Mild360
 Moderate175
 Severe153
 Medial temporal lobe atrophy1.56 (1.08)1.74 (1.21)NS
 Frontal atrophy1.66 (0.84)1.77 (0.96)NS

Values are mean (standard deviation) analyzed with Mann-Whitney U test, unless otherwise indicated.

N (%) significance values analyzed by χ2 test.

TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics and MRI Findings
Enlarge table

MRI Findings

When rating frontal lobe atrophy and medial temporal lobe atrophy, complete interrater agreement was reached in 72.7% and 70.9%, respectively; kappa values were 0.632 and 0.683, respectively. Rates of medial temporal lobe and frontal lobe atrophy did not differ significantly between the groups (Table 1).

Cognitive Functions

We found significant group differences between the SIVD and the non-SIVD-group on the following subtests: Semantic Verbal Fluency, Phonemic Verbal Fluency, Word-List Learning, and Trail-Making Tests A and B. No significant differences were obtained in the subtests Mini-Mental State Exam, Boston Naming Test, Word-List Delayed Recall, Word-List Recognition, Discriminability, Figure Copy, and Figure Recall (Table 2).

TABLE 2. Group Comparison: SIVD Versus Reference Group

No SIVD
SIVD
p
Mini-Mental State Exam–1.14 (1.45)–1.40 (1.92)NS
Semantic Verbal Fluency–0.28 (1.19)–0.87 (1.21)0.033
Phonemic Verbal Fluency0.13 (1.32)–0.42 (1.28)0.045
Boston Naming Test–0.12 (1.44)–0.34 (1.38)NS
Word-List Learning–1.12 (1.44)–1.71 (1.45)0.026
Word-List Delayed Recall–0.82 (1.13)–1.30 (1.34)0.056
Word-List Recognition–0.48 (1.60)–0.79 (1.76)NS
Discriminability–0.78 (1.33)–0.71 (1.47)NS
Figure Copy–0.19 (1.61)0.01 (1.91)NS
Figure Recall–0.38 (1.48)–0.55 (1.65)NS
Trail-Making Test A–0.20 (1.18)–0.88 (1.36)0.009
Trail-Making Test B0.11 (1.10)–0.50 (1.23)0.030

Values are mean (standard deviation). Significance values analyzed by Mann-Whitney U test.

TABLE 2. Group Comparison: SIVD Versus Reference Group
Enlarge table

Using univariate analysis, white-matter lesion load (as measured by the Fazekas scale) is significantly associated with performance in Semantic Verbal Fluency (β = −0.256; p=0.010) and Trail-Making Test A (β = −0.290; p=0.006).

Discussion

In our study population, neuropsychological measures of SIVD-patients showed significant differences in test scores of verbal fluency, verbal memory, speed of cognitive processing, and divided attention, when compared with the age- and gender-matched non-SIVD-group. We did not find significant differences in constructional praxis, figurative memory, verbal recognition memory, or semantic processing.

Subcortical periventricular and deep white-matter lesions may cause cognitive impairments even in patients not suffering from dementia. Jokinen and co-authors4 were the first to provide a comprehensive analysis of cognitive performance in SIVD. They found impairments in executive functions and delayed recall in SIVD patients. SIVD was not associated with mental speed, short-term memory, immediate recall, or visuospatial functions. Our findings are partly in line with Jokinen et al. We also found impairments in executive functions. Although there was a non-significant trend, we did not find significantly impaired scores on delayed recall in SIVD patients. Bilino et al.22 report impaired word-list learning performance in SIVD patients. This supports our findings. Kandiah and co-authors23 compared patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and SIVD patients suffering from mild dementia. SIVD patients with mild dementia had greater deficits in visuospatial functioning; working memory and visuomotor speed and were also more depressed than in AD patients. Executive-function tests in general did not distinguish the two groups.

Frontal lobe atrophy and consecutive frontal lobe dysfunctions are considered to play a key role leading to the clinical consequences of SIVD. The frontal lobe seems to be most severely affected by SIVD. Furthermore, white-matter lesions are more abundant in the frontal lobe. However, regardless of where in the brain white-matter lesions are located, they are associated with frontal hypometabolism and consecutive executive dysfunction.24 Among the cognitive domains impaired in patients with severe white-matter lesions, findings in frontal lobe functions (especially executive functions) seem to be the most consistent.4,10,25,26 Frontal executive functions include volition, planning, anticipation, control of socially inadequate behavior, and control of complex activities.15,16,2629 Impairments in executive functions are caused by disruptions of the dorso–lateral prefrontal–subcortical connections.27 The disruptions may be caused by lesions in the thalamus, the striatum, the globus pallidus, or the subcortical white matter.16,27 Previous studies have shown that white-matter lesions may cause brain atrophy,10,13,14,30,31 especially frontal atrophy.10 The findings of Mok et al.10 suggest a strong influence of the lateral fronto-orbital gyrus.

Our data suggest that SIVD is not associated with deficits in delayed memory. This contradicts the findings of Jokinen et al.,4 although Jokinen and co-authors hypothesize that verbal memory deficits might be mediated by executive deficits, and, therefore, they may reflect secondary impairment due to inefficient encoding and retrieval strategies.4 This hypothesis is supported by our finding that SIVD is related to impairment in verbal learning, but not delayed recognition. This indicates, that SIVD leads to an “executive-type” memory impairment.

This study has certain limitations. This is a retrospective analysis of a pre-selected group of patients, who were referred for planned carotid artery stenting because of asymptomatic carotid artery disease and subjective cognitive impairment. Bias due to multiple comparisons is possible. The neuropsychological test battery established by the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD-Plus)20,21 was designed as a tool for the detection of AD. It might not be suitable for the detection of cognitive deficits caused by vascular disease. Furthermore, handedness of patients was not assessed. The imaging criteria for SIVD, white-matter lesions, and signs of atrophy in the medial-temporal and frontal lobe were based on visual rating scales. The applied visual rating scales categorizing SIVD and frontal lobe and medial-temporal lobe atrophy are easily accessible in the clinical process.

From the Dept. of Neurology, Paracelsus Medizinische Privatuniversität Salzburg; the Dept. of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Klinikum rechts der Isar, Technische Universität München; Dept. of Neuroradiology, Klinikum rechts der Isar, Technische Universität München; the Neuroscience Institute, Christian Doppler Klinik, Paracelsus Medical University Salzburg, Austria; the Center for Neurocognitive Research, University of Salzburg, Salzburg, Austria; and the Division of Neuroradiology, Paracelsus Medizinische Privatuniversität Salzburg, Austria.
Send correspondence to Dr. Scherr; e-mail:
References

1 Román GC, Erkinjuntti T, Wallin A, et al.: Subcortical ischaemic vascular dementia. Lancet Neurol 2002; 1:426–436Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Erkinjuntti T: Vascular cognitive deterioration and stroke. Cerebrovasc Dis 2007; 24(Suppl 1):189–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Jokinen H, Kalska H, Ylikoski RLADIS group,, et al.: MRI-defined subcortical ischemic vascular disease: baseline clinical and neuropsychological findings: The LADIS Study. Cerebrovasc Dis 2009a; 27:336–344Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Jokinen H, Kalska H, Mäntylä R, et al.: Cognitive profile of subcortical ischaemic vascular disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006; 77:28–33Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Esiri MM, Wilcock GK, Morris JH: Neuropathological assessment of the lesions of significance in vascular dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1997; 63:749–753Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Pantoni L, Poggesi A, Inzitari D: Cognitive decline and dementia related to cerebrovascular diseases: some evidence and concepts. Cerebrovasc Dis 2009; 27(Suppl 1):191–196Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 LADIS Study Group: 2001–2011: a decade of the LADIS (Leukoaraiosis And DISability) Study: what have we learned about white-matter changes and small-vessel disease? Cerebrovasc Dis 2011; 32:577–588CrossrefGoogle Scholar

8 Erkinjuntti T: Diagnosis and management of vascular cognitive impairment and dementia. J Neural Transm Suppl 2002; 63:91–109MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Jokinen H, Kalska H, Ylikoski RLADIS group,, et al.: Longitudinal cognitive decline in subcortical ischemic vascular disease: the LADIS Study. Cerebrovasc Dis 2009b; 27:384–391Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Mok V, Wong KK, Xiong Y, et al.: Cortical and frontal atrophy are associated with cognitive impairment in age-related confluent white-matter lesion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011; 82:52–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Sabri O, Ringelstein EB, Hellwig D, et al.: Neuropsychological impairment correlates with hypoperfusion and hypometabolism but not with severity of white-matter lesions on MRI in patients with cerebral microangiopathy. Stroke 1999; 30:556–566Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Corbett A, Bennett H, Kos S: Cognitive dysfunction following subcortical infarction. Arch Neurol 1994; 51:999–1007Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Fein G, Di Sclafani V, Tanabe J, et al.: Hippocampal and cortical atrophy predict dementia in subcortical ischemic vascular disease. Neurology 2000; 55:1626–1635Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Chen Y, Chen X, Xiao W, et al.: Frontal lobe atrophy is associated with small-vessel disease in ischemic stroke patients. Clin Neurol Neurosurg 2009; 111:852–857Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Wolfe N, Linn R, Babikian VL, et al.: Frontal systems impairment following multiple lacunar infarcts. Arch Neurol 1990; 47:129–132Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Cummings JL: Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol 1993; 50:873–880Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Leys D, et al.: A semiquantitative rating scale for the assessment of signal hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurol Sci 1993; 114:7–12Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Pantoni L, Basile AM, Pracucci G, et al.: Impact of age-related cerebral white-matter changes on the transition to disability: the LADIS study: rationale, design, and methodology. Neuroepidemiology 2005; 24:51–62Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State:”a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al.: The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), Part I: clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1989; 39:1159–1165Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Welsh KA, Butters N, Mohs RC, et al.: The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD), Part V: a normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology 1994; 44:609–614Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Billino J, Luerssen J, von Renteln-Kruse W, et al.: Effects of subcortical vascular ischemic dementia and aging on negative and neutral word list learning. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2011; 31:188–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Kandiah N, Narasimhalu K, Lee J, et al.: Differences exist in the cognitive profile of mild Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical ischemic vascular dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2009; 27:399–403Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Tullberg M, Fletcher E, DeCarli C, et al.: White-matter lesions impair frontal lobe function regardless of their location. Neurology 2004; 63:246–253Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Pohjasvaara T, Mäntylä R, Ylikoski R, et al.: Clinical features of MRI-defined subcortical vascular disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2003; 17:236–242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Román GC, Royall DR: Executive control function: a rational basis for the diagnosis of vascular dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1999; 13(Suppl 3):S69–S80Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Mega MS, Cummings JL: Frontal-subcortical circuits and neuropsychiatric disorders. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 6:358–370LinkGoogle Scholar

28 Grigsby J, Kaye K, Shetterly SM, et al.: Prevalence of disorders of executive cognitive functioning among the elderly: findings from the San Luis Valley Health and Aging Study. Neuroepidemiology 2002; 21:213–220Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Royall DR: Executive cognitive impairment: a novel perspective on dementia. Neuroepidemiology 2000; 19:293–299Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Godin O, Maillard P, Crivello F, et al.: Association of white-matter lesions with brain atrophy markers: the three-city Dijon MRI study. Cerebrovasc Dis 2009; 28:177–184Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Appelman AP, Exalto LG, van der Graaf Y, et al.: White-matter lesions and brain atrophy: more than shared risk factors? a systematic review. Cerebrovasc Dis 2009; 28:227–242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar