The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
DepartmentsFull Access

Mild Depressive Symptoms are Associated With Enhanced Affective Theory of Mind in Nonclinical Adult Women

To the Editor: How do affective characteristics influence theory of mind (ToM), (i.e., the ability to ascribe mental states to others)? Major depression may impair the inference of others’ feelings1 (i.e., affective ToM), but in nonclinical subjects, mild levels of depression and anxiety may counterintuitively enhance this ability.2

We investigated this relationship by enrolling subjects from outpatients referred to our clinic for subjective memory complaints; inclusion criteria were age between 40 and 80 years, preserved cognitive functioning at the neuropsychological assessment, negative neurological and psychiatric history, any severe concomitant depressive and/or anxious symptomatology, any concomitant antidepressant use, or anxiolytic pharmacological treatment. Considering that only a few men were represented in our sample and that women have higher prevalence of affective disorders and better emotion processing abilities,3 we excluded men from the final sample of 40 women.

Affective ToM was assessed with the reading the mind in the eyes (RME),4 a task consisting of the presentation of photographs of the eye region of human faces; participants are required to choose which word best describes what the individual in the photograph is feeling); subjects also completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale (HAM-A) and were categorized as mildly depressed (BDI score >12) and not depressed (BDI score ≤12), and as moderately anxious (HAM-A score >17) and mildly anxious (HAM-A score ≤17).

The neuropsychiatric assessment produced these findings: mean age 65.80±9.40 years; mean education 9.50±4.0 years; mean BDI score 5.66±5.22 (10 mildly depressed subjects, 30 not depressed); mean HAM-A score 9.32±7.80 (29 subjects with mild anxiety, 11 with moderate anxiety); mean RME score was 21.33±4.47. A stepwise regression analysis with RME performance as a dependent variable and demographic features (age and education) and affective features (BDI score and HAM-A score) as independent variables was performed. Two models were generated: in the first model, the RME performance was associated with depression and education (F=4.730; p ≤0.01), and in the second model, the RME performance was associated only with depression (F=5.294; p <0.05). Although mildly depressed subjects and not depressed subjects had similar age, education, and anxiety level, t test comparison showed that mildly depressed subjects outperformed not depressed subjects in the RME (24.22±5.04 versus 20.48±3.99; t=–2.328; p <0.05); BDI score and RME performance positively correlated (r=0.333; p <0.05).

We found that mild depression may enhance affective ToM, as previously reported in a nonclinical sample of college students,2 indicating that depression severity may differentially affect social processing: mildly depressed subjects could be hypervigilant in their social information processing to gain control over their social environment; conversely, clinically depressed individuals may characterize their social relationships as hopeless and may stop paying attention to social information.5 This could support the hypothesis of a curvilinear relation between depressed mood and affective ToM, such that mildly depressed subjects show superior performance relative to not depressed subjects, whereas performance decreases as the severity of depression increases toward clinical levels.1 This finding needs further empirical confirmation in larger samples of subjects.

Dept. of Neuroscience, University of Pisa, Italy
Versilia Hospital, USL12 Toscana, Viareggio, Italy
Dept. of Neuroscience, University of Pisa, Italy
Send correspondence to Dr. Bonuccelli; e-mail:

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Lee L, Harkness KL, Sabbagh MA, et al.: Mental state decoding abilities in clinical depression. J Affect Disord 2005; 86:247–258Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Harkness K, Sabbagh M, Jacobson J, et al.: Enhanced accuracy of mental state decoding in dysphoric college students. Cogn Emotion 2005; 19:999–1025CrossrefGoogle Scholar

3 Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, et al.: The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42:241–251Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 McClure EB: A meta-analytic review of sex differences in facial expression processing and their development in infants, children, and adolescents. Psychol Bull 2000; 126:424–453Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Weary G, Edwards JA: Social cognition and clinical psychology: anxiety, depression and the processing of social information, in Handbook of social cognition. Edited by Wyer RS Jr, Srull TK. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1994, pp 289–338Google Scholar