The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
ArticlesFull Access

Neuropsychiatric Features of Fregoli Syndrome: An Individual Patient Meta-Analysis

Abstract

Objective:

Fregoli syndrome is a rare delusion characterized by the belief that familiar people are presenting themselves disguised as others to the affected person. Theories of delusional misidentification have suggested secondary (“organic”) underlying mechanisms; however, the pathoetiology of Fregoli syndrome has not been systematically evaluated. The investigators aimed to compare the neuropsychiatric features of Fregoli syndrome in primary and secondary psychoses.

Methods:

A systematic review and patient-level meta-analysis were conducted. Five databases were searched, ultimately yielding 83 studies that met selection criteria. Demographic characteristics, diagnosis, delusional content, neuropsychiatric features, investigations, and treatment information were extracted. Random-effects models were calculated, and odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were estimated.

Results:

A total of 119 patients with Fregoli syndrome were identified: 62 patients (52%) with primary psychosis, 50 (42%) with secondary psychosis, and seven (6%) with an unclear etiology. Patients with secondary psychosis were less likely than patients with primary psychosis to experience persecutory features (OR=0.26, 95% CI=0.10, 0.67; p=0.0057). Moreover, patients with secondary psychosis were more likely to experience Fregoli syndrome during a first episode of psychosis (OR=11.00, 95% CI=2.45, 49.39; p=0.0017). Right-sided brain lesions were more prominent than left-sided brain lesions in the total sample (χ2=5.0, df=1, p=0.025) and in the secondary psychosis subgroup (χ2=4.26, df=1, p=0.039).

Conclusions:

This is the first meta-analysis to investigate Fregoli syndrome. An estimated 42% of the reported cases involved a secondary etiology. These findings provide clinicians with a better understanding of the symptomatology of Fregoli syndrome and have potential to be applied in future research and clinical practice.

Fregoli syndrome is a delusion of misidentification, first described by Courbon and Fail in 1927 (1). It is characterized by the belief that a familiar person or people are presenting themselves to the affected individual disguised as others, meaning the appearance of individuals in the patient’s environment is accurately perceived, but these individuals, typically strangers, are misidentified as being familiar people in disguise (2). Commonly, a patient with Fregoli syndrome misidentifies several individuals as being the same familiar person in several different disguises, and the familiar person is often perceived to be intentionally following the affected individual (3).

Over the past 80 years, delusional misidentification syndromes like Fregoli syndrome have posed challenges to mental health professionals as a result of limited understanding of the etiology (4), lack of effective treatment (5), and concerns about risk (6). Earlier debates about misidentification delusions centered on whether they were best understood as “organic” (i.e., secondary to traditionally defined neurological conditions) rather than “nonorganic” (i.e., “functional” or primary psychiatric disorders) (7, 8). More recently, however, case-finding studies have noted that this simplistic classification does not account for the potential interactions between etiology and presentation (911). Indeed, there is emerging evidence of differences in the clinical presentation of misidentification delusions in the context of primary and secondary psychosis. For example, in a recent systematic review of Capgras syndrome, a related delusional misidentification syndrome where affected individuals misidentify familiar people as impostors, cases secondary to neurological disorder revealed lower rates of paranoid delusions and higher rates of cognitive impairment in memory and visuospatial domains (12).

Although Fregoli syndrome is phenomenologically distinct from other misidentification delusions (13) and likely arises from a characteristic etiology (2), to our knowledge, a comprehensive systematic review of the primary literature has never been performed. By using a patient-level meta-analytic approach, we sought to compare the neuropsychiatric features of patients with Fregoli syndrome that arose within a primary psychiatric disorder or secondary to a neurological disorder.

Methods

A systematic review of all published cases of Fregoli syndrome was conducted, followed by a patient-level meta-analysis comparing cases arising from a primary or secondary psychosis. This study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (14, 15) (for further details, see Table S1 in the online supplement). The meta-analysis was registered on the Open Science Framework.

Search Strategy and Selection Criteria

We searched five electronic databases (PubMed, Medline, Embase, PsycINFO, and Global Health) for articles published before November 10, 2020, using the following search terms with no date limits: “Fregoli delusion” OR “Fregoli syndrome” OR “delusions of misidentification” OR “delusional misidentification” OR “misidentification.” After removal of duplicates, each article was screened by title and abstract before the full paper was reviewed to confirm eligibility. Relevant systematic reviews and references of included cases were hand-searched for additional cases. The study search was conducted independently by two reviewers (M.T-D. and A.K.D.), and any discrepancies were resolved by a third independent reviewer (G.B.).

Inclusion criteria required articles to be written in English and to include one or more patients with Fregoli syndrome reported at a case level. In instances where one or more articles reported data for the same patient, the more detailed study was used. To determine whether patients met the clinical criteria for Fregoli syndrome, we assessed the reported delusional content against an adapted version of the criteria for identifying Capgras syndrome, as described by Bell and colleagues (9) (for further details, see Table S2 in the online supplement). Only cases where diagnostic certainty was “strong” or “possible” were included in the meta-analysis.

Data Collection and Synthesis

An electronic data extraction tool was developed and piloted. Data extraction from all eligible studies was performed by a researcher (M.T-D.). In addition, data from 20% of studies were independently extracted by a second researcher (A.K.D.) in parallel to that of the first researcher, and a third researcher (G.B.) arbitrated over any discrepancies. The following data were extracted from each study: demographic variables, diagnosis, delusional content, other psychiatric features, investigations, and treatment. For case reports, risk of bias was assessed with a modified version of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (16, 17) (for further details, see Table S3 in the online supplement). By using a 6-point scale, cases were rated as low risk of bias (scores of 5–6), medium risk of bias (scores of 2–4), or high risk of bias (scores of 0–1).

Statistical Analysis

First, summary statistics were computed for the whole sample. Thereafter, a patient-level meta-analysis was performed comparing patients with a primary and a secondary psychiatric disorder. Excluded from the meta-analyses were cases where the etiology was ambivalent or unknown or where insufficient data were reported (i.e., less than two main outcomes were reported). The primary analyses explored neuropsychiatric features, neuroimaging and neurophysiological abnormalities, and response to treatment. Secondary analyses were conducted to assess the content of the Fregoli delusion and neuroanatomical location of brain abnormalities.

A Wilcoxon rank-sum test was used to compare ages between the groups, and a Pearson’s chi-square test was used to evaluate sex differences. Frequencies and percentages were computed, and the differences between the primary and secondary psychosis groups were assessed with odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs), which were estimated by using random-effects models. Post hoc chi-square tests were conducted to compare the frequencies in right- and left-sided brain lesions in the total sample and in the secondary psychosis subgroup. To assess the robustness of the results, sensitivity analyses were used to explore the effects of risk of bias and effects of diagnosis certainty. Cases at high risk of bias and cases assessed as “possible” Fregoli syndrome according to the adapted Diagnosis Certainty Scale were excluded. All variables were dichotomous except age. The p value was set a priori as 0.05. To adjust for multiple comparisons in the secondary analyses, p values were adjusted by using Bonferroni correction (18). Statistical analyses were conducted in R, version 4.0.4 (19), with the psych (20), meta (21), and dplyr (22) packages. The data sets and full-analysis code are available through the Open Science Framework website (https://osf.io/qygbm/?view_only=f522b34f525c401e9cb0d68d1f890ed6).

Results

A total of 4,656 studies were identified from the study search, and 154 full-text articles were screened (for further details, see the PRISMA flowchart in Figure S1 in the online supplement). The study search yielded 83 studies, which reported 119 cases of Fregoli syndrome.

Patient data were extracted from case reports (N=61, 51%), case series (N=37, 31%), experimental studies (N=12, 10%), and abstracts (N=9, 8%). Overall, 62 patients (52%) had Fregoli syndrome in the context of primary psychoses, and 50 patients (42%) had Fregoli syndrome in the context of secondary psychoses. For the remaining cases (N=7, 6%), a mixed or unknown cause of psychosis was reported, and thus these cases were excluded from the meta-analysis.

We excluded from the meta-analysis cases for which insufficient data were provided (N=23, 19%; see the references in the online supplement), and thus a total of 89 patients with Fregoli syndrome (from 73 studies) were entered into the meta-analysis: 55 patients (62%) with a primary psychiatric disorder and 34 patients (38%) with a secondary cause (for case descriptions, see Table S7 in the online supplement). Of the cases included in the meta-analysis, 79 (89%) were rated “strong” in terms of diagnostic certainty.

Diagnoses of included patients are summarized in Table 1. The majority of patients with a primary psychiatric disorder had schizophrenia (N=32, 58%). In contrast, patients with a secondary disorder had a more even distribution of diagnoses, with stroke (N=9, 26%) and traumatic brain injury (N=7, 21%) being the most common. Most patients with Fregoli syndrome in the context of a secondary cause had a neurological disorder (N=26, 76%).

TABLE 1. Diagnoses of 89 patients with Fregoli syndrome included in the meta-analysis

DiagnosisN%
Primary psychosis55
 Schizophrenia3258
 Bipolar disorder713
 Psychotic depression59
 Schizoaffective disorder47
 Obsessive compulsive disorder12
 Other causesa611
Secondary psychosis34
 Stroke926
 Traumatic brain injury721
 Neurodegenerative disease515
 Substance-related412
 Systemic illness412
 Epilepsy39
 Other causes26

aAdditional details are described in Table S7 in the online supplement.

TABLE 1. Diagnoses of 89 patients with Fregoli syndrome included in the meta-analysis

Enlarge table

Risk of Bias Assessment

The majority of cases were rated as being at medium risk of bias (N=46, 52%), followed by high risk (N=31, 35%) and low risk (N=12, 13%). Individual scores are reported in Table S7 in the online supplement.

Age and Sex Differences

Patients in the secondary psychosis group were significantly older (median=60 years, interquartile range [IQR]=39 years) than patients in the primary psychosis group (median=33 years, IQR=17 years) when assessed with the Wilcoxon sign-rank test (W=510, p=0.002). There was no significant difference in the proportion of females in the primary and secondary psychosis groups (49% and 50%, respectively).

Neuropsychiatric Features

Meta-analysis revealed that patients with a secondary etiology were significantly more likely to experience Fregoli syndrome as part of a first episode of psychosis (OR=11.00, 95% CI=2.45, 49.39; z=3.13, p=0.0017). In contrast, they were less likely to experience persecutory delusions (OR=0.26, 95% CI=0.10, 0.67; z=−2.76, p=0.0057). All other neuropsychiatric features showed no differences between groups (Figure 1; see also Table S4 in the online supplement).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1. Characteristics of 89 patients with Fregoli syndrome with primary and secondary psychosisa

aAnalyses were based on investigation findings, neuropsychiatric domains, psychotic features, and response to treatment. CT, computed tomography; EEG, electroencephalogram.

Neuroimaging Abnormalities

In total, neuroimaging results detected by either computed tomography or MRI were reported for 21 patients (38%) with a primary psychotic disorder and 23 patients (68%) with a secondary cause. Of these patients, four (19%) in the primary psychosis group and 19 (83%) in the secondary psychosis group had neuroimaging abnormalities reported (Table 2). This difference was statistically significant (OR=20.19, 95% CI=4.36, 93.47; z=3.84, p=0.0001). We explored the neuroanatomical location of brain abnormalities detected. Almost three-quarters of patients with secondary psychosis had right-hemispheric lesions (N=14, 74%). In addition, more than one-half of patients with secondary psychosis had a frontal lobe abnormality (N=10, 53%). In contrast, the majority of patients in the primary psychosis group had bilateral lesions (N=3, 75%). Random-effects models revealed no significant associations between psychosis etiology and MRI abnormality location (Table 2). In a direct comparison of right- versus left-sided lesion frequencies, right-sided lesions were more common across the whole sample (χ2=5.0, df=1, p=0.025), and this remained the case in the secondary psychosis group (χ2=4.26, df=1, p=0.039). Right-sided lesions were more common than left-sided lesions in the primary psychosis group, although the low total count in this group (one patient with a right-sided lesion and zero patients with left-sided lesions) excluded statistical analysis.

TABLE 2. Neuroimaging abnormalities among patients with Fregoli syndrome with primary or secondary psychosis

Total (N=23)Primary psychosis (N=4)Secondary psychosis (N=19)
Lesion locationN%N%N%pa
Right side156512514740.65
Bilateral9393756320.95
Left side522005260.99
Frontal lobe125225010530.99
Temporal lobe5223752110.13
Temporoparietal417004210.99
Frontoparietal313003160.99

aThe p values were adjusted by using Bonferroni correction; unadjusted p values are reported in Table S5 in the online supplement.

TABLE 2. Neuroimaging abnormalities among patients with Fregoli syndrome with primary or secondary psychosis

Enlarge table

A meta-analysis based on patients with reported neuroimaging results in the primary psychosis group (N=4) and the secondary psychosis group (N=19) revealed no significant differences in neuropsychiatric features between groups (for further details, see Figure S2 in the online supplement).

Delusional Content

The majority of patients with a primary (N=34/54, 63%) or secondary (N=18/30, 60%) disorder experienced more than one Fregoli delusion (i.e., patients who identified more than one identity appearing in the guise of others). The content of the Fregoli delusions reported is summarized in Table 3. Patients with a secondary psychosis were more likely to mistakenly identify family members than patients with a primary psychosis (OR=4.98, 95% CI=1.96, 12.65; z=3.37, p=0.007). Between groups, there were no significant differences in the categories of people from the environment who were being misidentified.

TABLE 3. Delusional content reported among patients with Fregoli syndrome with primary and secondary psychosis

Total (N=86)aPrimary psychosis (N=52)Secondary psychosis (N=34)
Delusional contentN%N%N%pb
Person in the environment
 Hospital staff and police4350234420590.99
 Stranger252919376180.64
 Family member192211218240.99
 Acquaintancec1012815260.99
 Television character3324130.99
Identity perceived
 Family member3642142722650.007
 Acquaintancec252917338240.99
 Romantic interestd13151019390.99
 Famous person89713130.99
 Person related to trauma4536130.99

aThe total number of patients who reported delusional content.

bThe p values were adjusted by using Bonferroni correction; unadjusted p values are reported in Table S6 in the online supplement.

cAcquaintances included neighbors, friends, and school- and work-related people.

dPeople with a romantic link also included ex-partners.

TABLE 3. Delusional content reported among patients with Fregoli syndrome with primary and secondary psychosis

Enlarge table

Sensitivity Analyses

Sensitivity analyses excluding cases at high risk of bias revealed no change in significant findings in the primary outcomes. However, the association between secondary psychosis and misidentification of family members became nonsignificant in the secondary analysis. Sensitivity analyses excluding cases rated as possibly Fregoli syndrome did not lead to any changes in the significant findings.

Discussion

In this study, we explored the differences in neuropsychiatric features associated with patients experiencing Fregoli syndrome related to a primary or secondary psychotic disorder. A total of 119 reported cases of Fregoli syndrome were found in the English literature since the first published description in 1927 (1), which averages 1.3 published cases per year. Of the reported patients, 42% (N=50) experienced a Fregoli delusion in the context of a secondary cause, most commonly a neurological disorder.

To our knowledge, this is the first comprehensive meta-analysis to specifically examine Fregoli syndrome. However, previous research examined the neuropsychiatric features of delusions of misidentification more broadly. An earlier review of 260 case reports of delusions of misidentification explored the most frequent associated neuropsychiatric features, which included paranoid symptoms, depression, auditory hallucinations, and aggressive behavior (23). In a more recent systematic review of Capgras syndrome, which is the most common type of misidentification delusion, a significant difference in the neuropsychiatric features was observed between patients with a primary and secondary etiology (12). Specifically, patients with a secondary etiology were more likely to experience visual hallucinations, to exhibit neuroimaging and neurophysiological abnormalities, and to have memory and visuospatial impairments (12). Moreover, they were less likely to have formal thought disorder, to experience auditory hallucinations, to experience other misidentification and paranoid delusions, and to show violent behavior.

Among patients who underwent neuroimaging, it was notable that right-sided lesions were particularly common. In addition, there was an overrepresentation of patients with lesions in the frontal lobe. The preponderance of right and frontal neuroanatomical lesions in patients with misidentification delusions has been reported in previous studies (24, 25). Darby and Prasad (26) investigated a sample of 61 patients with various delusions of misidentification and found that 63% had right frontal lesions and that the frequency was significantly higher than that of lesions in other lobes or in the left hemisphere (26). Furthermore, in a recent MRI study that examined brain connectivity of lesions related to delusions of misidentifications, the investigators found that almost all the patients in the sample (N=17) had lesions in brain areas functionally connected to the right frontal cortex (27) and that this was specific to misidentification delusions compared with other neurological syndromes. This highlights the critical importance of the right frontal regions that were found in our sample of patients with Fregoli syndrome. Dysfunction in the right hemisphere and frontal lobe is associated with the misplacement of familiarity, as well as with faults in reality monitoring and memory integration (28). It is plausible that these cognitive impairments may result in incoherent and false explanations, inducing Fregoli syndrome and other delusions of misidentification.

Persecutory features associated with Fregoli syndrome were more likely to occur among patients with primary psychosis than among those with secondary psychosis. Interestingly, this association was also reported in Capgras syndrome in a study conducted by Pandis and colleagues (12). This finding may be explained by the high proportion of patients in the primary psychosis group who had schizophrenia, in which paranoid delusions are one of the most common symptoms (29). Additionally, our results showed that patients with secondary psychosis were older, which is also consistent with findings in studies of Capgras syndrome (12), as well as with several age-associated causes of delusions of misidentification, including stroke and neurodegenerative disorders (30).

One of the key findings from the present meta-analysis was that almost one-half of all patients with Fregoli syndrome had a secondary cause of psychosis. This suggests that clinicians should have a high index of suspicion for a neurological etiology, rather than a primary psychiatric etiology, for any patient presenting with Fregoli syndrome and therefore request relevant neuroimaging investigations, such as MRI. Clinicians should be particularly mindful of older patients and patients presenting with a Fregoli delusion in the context of a first psychotic episode, where neurological causality is likely to be more prevalent. However, we sound a note of caution because reviews of Capgras delusion based on case reports and case series have reported high levels of neurological etiologies (12) and forensic risks (31), which have not been found in subsequent case-finding studies (9, 10), potentially suggesting that certain features may be overrepresented in the published literature. Fregoli syndrome may be a phenomenological marker for secondary etiology; however, future case-finding studies remain key to answering this question.

Strengths and Limitations

There are several strengths of this meta-analysis. First, the study comprised the largest synthesis of cases of Fregoli syndrome, to our knowledge, and therefore provides the most comprehensive and up-to-date review of the literature. Second, a case-level approach yielded a highly granular level of detail, including exploration of descriptive psychopathology. Third, we were able to evaluate the diagnostic certainty of cases based on reported phenomenology, providing a high degree of confidence regarding caseness. Fourth, sensitivity analyses indicated that the results were robust to risk of bias and diagnosis certainty.

There are, however, certain limitations to this study. Most cases were extracted from case reports and case series, which generally constitute low levels of evidence. Indeed, most cases were at medium or high risk of bias. Additionally, the inherent nature of case reports and case series meant that these cases were likely to be overly representative of unusual or extreme presentations. This could have led to an overestimation of patients with a secondary cause of psychosis. Furthermore, there was a lack of consistency in the reporting of clinical features across studies, which led to several missing data points, lowering the statistical power and increasing the risk of a type 1 error. In particular, the number of patients with a primary cause of psychosis who reported neuroimaging results was very low. These issues may have introduced publication bias, as well as outcome reporting bias, and therefore caution is warranted in generalizing our results.

Future Directions

Case-finding studies will be required to determine the prevalence of primary and secondary causes of Fregoli syndrome. This information, combined with sensitivity and specificity, would allow positive and negative predictive values to be estimated for phenomenology as a potential guide to secondary causation. However, due to the rarity of Fregoli syndrome, recruitment is likely to present a substantial challenge to such an endeavor. The use of electronic health records may be a fruitful alternative (9). In addition, structural and functional neuroimaging studies are necessary to further investigations regarding the lateralization of abnormalities to advance our understanding of the neural correlates of Fregoli syndrome.

Conclusions

Fregoli syndrome is a rare delusion of misidentification commonly associated with secondary (“organic”) causes in the published literature. We found that patients with a secondary cause of psychosis were more likely to be older, present as part of a first episode of psychosis, and have neuroimaging abnormalities than patients with a primary (“nonorganic”) cause of psychosis. These findings are informative to our understanding of potential mechanisms underlying Fregoli syndrome. Furthermore, the results suggest that clinicians should have a high index of suspicion for a secondary cause when a patient presents with Fregoli syndrome, with MRI indicated in all cases.

Section of Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, London (Teixeira-Dias, Dadwal, Blackman); Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London (Bell); South London and Maudsley National Health Service Foundation Trust, London (Bell, Blackman).
Send correspondence to Dr. Blackman ().

Preliminary findings were presented as a poster at the Faculty of Neuropsychiatry Annual Conference (presented online September 17 and 18, 2020) and at the British Neuropsychiatry Association Annual Meeting (presented online March 11, 2021).

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Courbon P, Fail G: Syndrome of the illusion of Fregoli and schizophrenia [in French]. Bull la Soc Clin Med Ment 1927; 20:121–125Google Scholar

2 Langdon R, Connaughton E, Coltheart M, et al.: The Fregoli delusion: a disorder of person identification and tracking. Top Cogn Sci 2014; 6:615–631Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Thibierge S, Morin C: Which identification is disturbed in misidentification syndromes? a structural analysis of Fregoli and Capgras syndromes. J Mind Behav 2016; 37:1–13Google Scholar

4 Ventriglio A, Bhugra D, De Berardis D, et al.: Capgras and Fregoli syndromes: delusion and misidentification. Int Rev Psychiatry 2020; 32:391–395Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Atta K, Forlenza N, Gujski M, et al.: Delusional misidentification syndromes: separate disorders or unusual presentations of existing DSM-IV categories? Psychiatry 2006; 3:56–61MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Ashraf N, Antonius D, Sinkman A, et al.: Fregoli syndrome: an underrecognized risk factor for aggression in treatment settings. Case Rep Psychiatry 2011; 2011:351824MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Forstl H, Almeida OP, Iacoponi E: Capgras delusion in the elderly: the evidence for a possible organic origin. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1991; 6:845–852CrossrefGoogle Scholar

8 Joseph AB: Capgras syndrome. Br J Psychiatry 1986; 148:749–750Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Bell V, Marshall C, Kanji Z, et al.: Uncovering Capgras delusion using a large-scale medical records database. BJPsych Open 2017; 3:179–185Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Currell EA, Werbeloff N, Hayes JF, et al.: Cognitive neuropsychiatric analysis of an additional large Capgras delusion case series. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2019; 24:123–134Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Fleminger S, Burns A, Burns A: The delusional misidentification syndromes in patients with and without evidence of organic cerebral disorder: a structured review of case reports. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 33:22–32Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Pandis C, Agrawal N, Poole N, et al.: Capgras’ delusion: a systematic review of 255 published cases. Psychopathology 2019; 52:161–173Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Barrelle A, Luauté J-P: Capgras syndrome and other delusional misidentification syndromes. Front Neurol Neurosci 2018; 42:35–43Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, et al.: The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate healthcare interventions: explanation and elaboration. J Clin Epidemiol 2009; 62:e1–e34Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, et al.: Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med 2009; 6:e100097CrossrefGoogle Scholar

16 Murad MH, Sultan S, Haffar S, et al.: Methodological quality and synthesis of case series and case reports. BMJ Evid Based Med 2018; 23:60–63Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, et al.: The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Ottawa, Ontario, Ottawa Hospital, 2013. http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp. Accessed Aug 6, 2020 Google Scholar

18 Jafari M, Ansari-Pour N: Why, when and how to adjust your p-values? Cell J 2019; 20:604–607MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 R Core Team R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, European Environment Agency, 2021. https://www.r-project.org Google Scholar

20 Revelle W: psych: procedures for personality and psychological research. 2020. https://cran.r-project.org/package=psych Google Scholar

21 Balduzzi S, Rucker G, Schwarzer G: How to perform a meta-analysis with R: a practical tutorial. Evid Based Ment Health 2019; 22:153–160 Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Wickham H, François R, Henry L, et al: dplyr: a grammar of data manipulation, R package, Version 0.4Google Scholar

23 Förstl H, Almeida OP, Owen AM, et al.: Psychiatric, neurological and medical aspects of misidentification syndromes: a review of 260 cases. Psychol Med 1991; 21:905–910Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Feinberg TE, Deluca J, Giacino JT, et al.: Right-hemisphere pathology and the self: delusional misidentification and reduplication; in The Lost Self: Pathologies of the Brain and Identity. Edited by Feinberg TE, Keenan JP. New York, Oxford University Press, 2005, pp 100–130. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195173413.003.0008 CrossrefGoogle Scholar

25 Joyce EM: Organic psychosis: the pathobiology and treatment of delusions. CNS Neurosci Ther 2018; 24:598–603Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Darby R, Prasad S: Lesion-related delusional misidentification syndromes: a comprehensive review of reported cases. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2016; 28:217–222LinkGoogle Scholar

27 Darby RR, Laganiere S, Pascual-Leone A, et al.: Finding the imposter: brain connectivity of lesions causing delusional misidentifications. Brain 2017; 140:497–507Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Devinsky O: Delusional misidentifications and duplications: right brain lesions, left brain delusions. Neurology 2009; 72:80–87Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Peralta V, Cuesta MJ: Characteristics and clinical correlates of dimensions of delusional experience in schizophrenia and delusional disorder. Schizophr Res 2016; 176:404–410Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Perini G, Carlini A, Pomati S, et al.: Misidentification delusions: prevalence in different types of dementia and validation of a structured questionnaire. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2016; 30:331–337Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Fishbain DA, Szulecka TK: Dangerous delusions: violence and the misidentification syndromes. Br J Psychiatry 1988; 152:859–860Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar