The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.11.3.395

Abstract

This study examined the verbal and design fluency abilities of 25 subclinical obsessive-compulsive (OC) subjects and 27 noncompulsive control subjects. As hypothesized, the OC group showed reduced design fluency, and design fluency was also negatively correlated with obsessionality. These results provide further evidence for the involvement of the right corticostriatal systems in the mediation of OC behaviors, extending the findings to individuals with subclinical symptoms.

Current neurobiological models of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) involve dysfunctions in the orbitofrontal and anterior cingulate cortices, as well as the striatum and thalamus.1 Neuropsychological studies have been consistent with subtle deficits involving these structures, reporting deficits in visuospatial abilities, nonverbal memory, and executive functions in the presence of intact language, verbal memory, and intellectual abilities.2

An important question is whether those findings can be extended to subjects who do not meet diagnostic criteria for OCD. The study of subclinical samples, selected on the basis of self-administered instruments, has permitted investigation of the neuropsychological correlates of obsessive-compulsive (OC) phenomena; this approach has the potential benefit of overcoming some of the methodological limitations associated with clinical samples, such as medication or symptomatic state effects that may affect neuropsychological performance.3,4 The results of those studies in subclinical samples have been consistent with current models of OCD. For example, a recent study reported that subclinical OC subjects show difficulties in solving the Tower of Hanoi puzzle, a measure of executive functioning that requires manipulation of spatial information.4

Fluency tasks are measures of executive functioning that are sensitive to the functioning of the frontal lobes.5 There is evidence that performance on design fluency tasks may be particularly sensitive to right frontal cortical functions6 and that verbal fluency may be especially sensitive to left frontal functions.5

The present study systematically examined the verbal and design fluency abilities of a subclinical OC sample in order to further characterize the neuropsychological profile associated with nonclinical obsessive-compulsive phenomena. On the basis of previous neuropsychological studies of OCD, we predicted that subclinical OC subjects would show reductions on design fluency, as well as on verbal fluency tasks involving a set-shifting component. Such results would provide support for a dimensional model of obsessive-compulsive phenomena with an assumed continuity between normal and abnormal obsessions and compulsions.

METHODS

Following the methodology from previous studies,3,4 we recruited an independent cohort of 52 subjects from a new pool of 476 undergraduates at the Universitat de Barcelona on the basis of their scores on the Spanish version of the Padua Inventory (PI), a well-validated measure of OC symptoms.7 Completing the present study were 25 subclinical obsessive-compulsive subjects (21 women, 4 men; mean age=18.7±0.7 years; 4 left-handed) who scored higher than 1 standard deviation above the mean (PI≥86; mean=101.4±13.8) and 27 noncompulsive control subjects (23 women, 4 men; mean age=19.1±1.3 years; 1 left-handed; 33≤PI≤55; mean=41.9±14.2; means and standard deviations). Exclusion criteria were history of OCD or other psychiatric disorders. Three subjects met DSM-IV criteria for OCD and hence were excluded from the OC group; 1 subject was excluded from the control group because of missing data. Groups did not differ on gender or handedness. Preliminary analyses showed no effect of those variables on the overall results, and they were not further examined.

Three fluency tests were administered to the sample: two verbal fluency tests and one design fluency test. In the FAS test,8 the subjects were asked to generate as many words as possible beginning with the letters F, A, and S for 90 seconds. In the Category Alternation Test (CAT), following Newcombe's9 procedure, subjects had to name as many different items as possible from two categories of objects (animals and fruits) for 90 seconds. The Design Fluency Test (DFT), based on the test developed by Jones-Gotman and Milner,6 consists of two conditions. In the free condition, the subjects had to invent drawings that are not actual objects or nameable abstract forms (e.g., geometric shapes). Subjects were given 4 minutes to make up as many different kinds of such drawings possible. In the fixed condition, subjects were asked to generate as many different figures as possible, each contained in two vertical parallel lines, for 4 minutes.

In addition, the subjects were given the Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) as a measure of general nonverbal intelligence. The State subscale of the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) and the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) were also administered to control for anxiety and depression at the time of testing. Subjects were tested by two trained neuropsychologists. Blindness was assessed by asking the raters to guess the subject's group membership. Kappas were low (0.02 to 0.1).

Statistical analyses included multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs) comparing the two groups with the four fluency tasks as dependent variables and the state measures (STAI, BDI) as covariates; one-way analyses of variance; Mann-Whitney U-tests; and Pearson correlations.

RESULTS

The OC and control groups did not differ on the RAPM (means±SD: OC: 23.2±6.2; control: 22.4±4.7; F=0.24, df=1,50, not significant). OC subjects tended to be more anxious (STAI: 23.08±11.5) and depressed (BDI: 9.7±6.9) than control subjects (STAI: 16.6±5.4; BDI: 5.6±5.9) at the moment of the testing (STAI: Mann-Whitney U=238, P=0.06; BDI: F=5.24, df=1,50, P=0.02).

MANOVA with the four fluency tasks as dependent variables revealed a significant multivariate effect (Table 1). Univariate Fs showed that between-group differences were due to performance on the CAT and the free condition of the DFT. However, introduction of the STAI and the BDI as covariates in the model reduced group differences on the CAT to nonsignificance (F=1.98, df=1,47, not significant) because of the effect of state anxiety (β=–0.35, t=–2.3, P=0.02). The multivariate effect, however, became more pronounced (F=3.17, df=4,44, P=0.02), as did the univariate F for the DFT free condition (F=9.91, df=1,47, P=0.003). The PI total score was significantly negatively correlated with the DFT performance (Pearson r=–0.35, P=0.01, two-tailed), but not with the CAT (r=–0.17, not significant).

DISCUSSION

As previously reported in both subclinical and clinical samples,4,10 no differences were found on the phonemic fluency task (FAS). As predicted, subclinical OC subjects showed a reduced fluency on the Category Alternation Test, which involves set-shifting abilities in addition to verbal fluency. However, those differences disappeared when state anxiety was controlled as a covariate. Moreover, the CAT performance was not significantly correlated with obsessionality but was correlated with state anxiety. Reduction in semantic category alternation in OCD was reported by Harvey,11 but not in a better-controlled study by Head et al.12 It is possible that category alternation performance is mediated by state anxiety and does not constitute a basic cognitive deficit in OCD.

The results confirmed the hypothesis of a reduced design fluency in subclinical OC subjects. Design Fluency Test performance was significantly negatively correlated with obsessionality. The fact that only the DFT free condition showed between-group differences is consistent with recent findings suggesting that OCD patients have special difficulties in organizing nonstructured material.13 Furthermore, the fixed condition is less sensitive to frontal dysfunction than the free condition.5

The finding of a reduced design fluency may suggest a subtle dysfunction of the right corticostriatal systems in subclinical OC subjects. An alternative interpretation of the data, although the two are not mutually exclusive, is that the differences in performance between groups might reflect differences in cognitive style rather than deficits in those systems. The results are consistent with the current neurobiological models of OCD1 and provide further support for a dimensional model of obsessive-compulsive phenomena that assumes a continuity between normal and abnormal OC behavior.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This work was partially supported by Grants FISss 94/0908 and DGICYT PM95/104. The first author's work was funded by the Spanish Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia.

TABLE 1. Fluency tests: means, standard deviations, and significance levels for the multivariate analysis of variance comparing subclinical obsessive-compulsive (OC) and control groups

Received October 16, 1998; revised February 12, 1999; accepted February 22, 1999. From Departament de Psiquiatria i Psicobiologia Clínica, Universitat de Barcelona, Passeig de la Vall d'Hebron, 171, E-08035 Barcelona, Catalunya, Spain. Send correspondence to Dr. Mataix-Cols at the above address; e-mail:
References

1 Rauch SL, Bates JF, Grachev ID: Obsessive-compulsive disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 1997; 6:365–381CrossrefGoogle Scholar

2 Savage CR: Neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: research findings and treatment implications, in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorders: Practical Management, edited by Jenike MA, Baer L, Minichiello WE. St. Louis, MO, Mosby, 1998, pp 254–275Google Scholar

3 Mataix-Cols D, Junqué C, Vallejo J, et al: Hemispheric functional imbalance in a sub-clinical obsessive-compulsive sample assessed by the Continuous Performance Test, Identical Pairs version. Psychiatry Res 1997; 72:115–126Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Mataix-Cols D, Junqué C, Sànchez-Turet M, et al: Neuropsychological functioning in a sub-clinical obsessive-compulsive sample. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:898–904Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd edition. New York, Oxford University Press, 1995Google Scholar

6 Jones-Gotman M, Milner B: Design fluency: the invention of nonsense drawings after focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychologia 1977; 15:653–674Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Sanavio E: Obsessions and compulsions: the Padua Inventory. Behav Res Ther 1988; 26:169–177Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Spreen O, Benton AL: Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia (NCCEA). Victoria, Australia, University of Victoria Neuropsychology Laboratory, 1977Google Scholar

9 Newcombe F: Missile Wounds of the Brain. London, Oxford University Press, 1969Google Scholar

10 Christensen KJ, Won Kim S, Dysken MW, et al: Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 31:4–18Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Harvey NS: Impaired cognitive set-shifting in obsessive compulsive neurosis. IRCS Medical Science 1986; 14:936–937Google Scholar

12 Head D, Bolton D, Hymas N: Deficit in cognitive shifting ability in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1989; 25:929–937Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Savage CR, Baer L, Keuthen NJ, et al: Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:905–916Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar