The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Letter to the Editor: The closing-in phenomenon, that is, the tendency to close in on a model while performing a constructive task, has been reported in Alzheimer’s disease (AD),1 frontotemporal dementia,2 and vascular dementia.3 Recent studies suggested that closing-in probably reflects a compensatory strategy to overcome basic visuospatial dysfunctions involved in the preliminary visuo-perceptual analysis and/or in the on-line maintenance of the visual representation of the model while performing a copying/drawing task.4 Although visuospatial impairment is also a common cognitive feature in synucleinopathies, such as Parkinson’s disease dementia (PDD) and Lewy-body dementia (LBD),5 closing-in has never been reported in these disorders. We report on four cases of synucleinopathies (three PDD and one DLB) in which we detected closing-in phenomena.

Case 1: “GM” is a 76-year-old, right-handed man. He received a diagnosis of Parkinson’s disease (PD) when he was 67 years old. When he was 74, he began to show mild cognitive difficulties (MMSE: 25/30), especially related to executive functions (Frontal Assessment Battery [FAB] score: 12/18). Two years later, he developed a clear dementia associated with PD (levodopa daily dose: 940 mg/day; UPDRS-II/III: 15/32; Hoehn & Yahr [H&Y] rating, 3). GM then showed impaired cognitive status (MMSE: 20/30), with severe impairments of executive functions (FAB score: 8/18) and visuospatial functions (Benton’s Judgment of Line Orientation [JOL] score: 12/30; Raven's Colored Progressive Matrices [CPM], 16/36). During constructional praxis tasks, he showed closing-in of the “overlap” subtype.3

Case 2: “DM” is a 71-year-old, right-handed man. He received a diagnosis of PD when he was 64 years old. When he was 69, he began to show mild cognitive difficulties (MMSE: 23/30), especially related to executive functions (FAB: 11/18) and visuospatial functions (Benton’s JOL: 18/30; Raven's CPM: 19/36). One year later, he developed visual hallucinations, and, after 2 years, he developed PDD (levodopa daily dose: 675 mg/day; UPDRS-II/III: 19/24; H&Y: 3). DM presented a worsened cognitive status (MMSE: 16/30) and a severe and worsened impairment of executive (FAB: 7/18) and visuospatial functions (Benton’s JOL: 13/30; Raven's CPM: 15/36). During constructional praxis tasks, he showed closing-in of the “adherent” subtype.3

Case 3: “QB” is a 79-year-old, right-handed man. He received a diagnosis of PD when he was 74 years old. He shows cognitive difficulties (MMSE: 21/30), especially related to executive (FAB: 8/18) and visuospatial functions (Benton’s JOL: 18/30), that led to the diagnosis of PDD (levodopa daily dose: 750 mg/day; UPDRS-II/III: 10/31; H&Y: 2.5). During constructional praxis tasks, he showed closing-in of the “overlap” subtype.3

Case 4: “SD” is an 81-year-old, right-handed, married man. Six months ago, he began to show fluctuating mild cognitive impairment and visual hallucinations; then a mild parkinsonism also appeared. The DaT-SCAN- SPECT revealed bilateral putaminal degeneration. His global cognitive status was impaired (MMSE: 22/30), especially related to executive (FAB: 8/18) and visuospatial functions (Benton’s JOL: 15/30; Raven's CPM: 19/36). He received a diagnosis of LBD. During constructional praxis tasks, he showed closing-in of the “adherent” subtype.3

These cases show that the closing-in phenomenon may be present also in synucleinopathies associated with dementia, as PDD and DLB, in which visuospatial impairment is often a cognitive hallmark.

Dept. of Neuroscience, University of Pisa, Italy Neurology Unit, USL of Viareggio, Italy
Neurology Unit, USL of Viareggio, Italy
Dept. of Neuroscience, University of Pisa, Italy Neurology Unit, USL of Viareggio, Italy
Neurology Unit, USL of Viareggio, Italy
Dept. of Neuroscience, University of Pisa, Italy Neurology Unit, USL of Viareggio, Italy
Correspondence: Prof. Ubaldo Bonuccelli, Dept. of Neuroscience, University of Pisa, Italy; e-mail:

Conflict of interest relative to the research covered in this manuscript: none.

References

1 Ambron E, McIntosh RD, Allaria F, et al.: A large-scale retrospective study of closing-in behavior in Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009; 15:787–792Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Ambron E, Allaria F, McIntosh RD, et al.: Closing-in behaviour in fronto-temporal dementia. J Neurol 2009; 256:1004–1006Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Kwak YT: “Closing-in” phenomenon in Alzheimer’s disease and subcortical vascular dementia. BMC Neurol 2004; 4:3Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Serra L, Fadda L, Perri R, et al.: The closing-in phenomenon in the drawing performance of Alzheimer’s disease patients: a compensation account. Cortex 2010; 46:1031–1036Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Kurita A, Murakami M, Takagi S, et al.: Visual hallucinations and altered visual information-processing in Parkinson disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Mov Disord 2010; 25:167–171Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar