The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticlesFull Access

Rostral Anterior Cingulate Activity in Major Depressive Disorder: State or Trait Marker of Responsiveness to Medication?

Abstract

High rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) activity has been shown to predict antidepressant treatment response; however, it is unclear whether this is a fixed versus variable marker of responsiveness. The authors measured rACC theta current density in 22 subjects 5 weeks before and again immediately before 5 weeks of blinded treatment with sertraline. Mixed-effects regression analysis found that the relationship between response and rACC activity depended significantly on the timing of the rACC assessment; rACC activity measured immediately before treatment was a significantly better predictor of response. rACC activity may constitute a variable “state” indicator of responsiveness to antidepressants.

Converging lines of evidence suggest that the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) plays a crucial role in the neurobiology of major depressive disorder (MDD) and that activity in this region predicts response to antidepressant treatment. Patients suffering from MDD who respond to medication show elevated pretreatment activity in the rACC relative to non-responders.16 The relationship between rACC activity and treatment response has been demonstrated by multiple imaging modalities, including positron emission tomography (PET),1,3 and with quantitative electroencephalographic (qEEG) source localization, using low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA).2,4,5,7

Elevated resting state rACC activity has been hypothesized to reflect a greater ability to engage in adaptive self-referential processing, thereby conferring better prognosis for antidepressant treatment outcome.6 It is unknown whether this neurophysiologic marker remains fixed, or whether resting state rACC activity may change over the course of a single depressive episode, thus providing a variable or “state” marker of responsiveness. This question is of interest because, to the extent that rACC activity may constitute a state marker, there may be critical times in the course of illness in which treatment would be more likely to be effective. It also might be possible to alter rACC activity via the manipulation of state, for example, through cognitive interventions, mindfulness practices, or other methods of self-regulation, in order to enhance response to treatment.

Previous reports have not examined rACC activity on more than one occasion before treatment. Therefore, it is not known whether the relationship between rACC activity level and clinical response depends on the time of the rACC assessment. If rACC activity varies over time and reflects a state marker of responsiveness, then we would expect that the relationship between rACC activity and antidepressant response depends on the timing of the rACC measurement. Measurements closer in time to the beginning of antidepressant treatment would better reflect the “state” of the individual at treatment initiation, and therefore would be expected to be better predictors of outcome of a course of antidepressant treatment.

The current study examined rACC activity at two time-points: 5 weeks before the beginning of antidepressant treatment, and again immediately preceding the start of treatment. We hypothesized that if rACC represents a state marker of responsiveness, then the relationship between antidepressant response and rACC activity would depend on the timing of the rACC measurement.

Methods

Study Design

We examined brain electrical activity in 22 subjects who had failed to respond to 5 weeks of double-blinded placebo treatment and who then were crossed over to blinded treatment with antidepressant medication. EEG measurements were taken before the placebo treatment phase (5 weeks before crossover to medication) and again immediately before medication treatment was initiated. This study design allowed us to examine rACC brain functioning measured on two separate occasions before the start of antidepressant treatment, under conditions where neither EEG was confounded by psychotropic medications.

All subjects had been enrolled in a two-phase, placebo-controlled treatment trial of MDD (ClinicalTrials.gov #NCT00229476) primarily designed to provide insights into the placebo effect in MDD. The first phase of the trial consisted of an initial week of single-blinded placebo treatment, followed by 5 weeks of double-blinded treatment with placebo or medication (sertraline), with 3:1 randomization favoring placebo. In the second phase of the study, placebo non-responders from the first phase of the study (17-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression [Ham-D–17] >10) underwent a blinded crossover to medication.

Double-blind treatment was maintained throughout the study. Subjects were told that they would receive placebo or medication at the outset of the study, that all subjects would be on placebo at some point, and, that changes to their medication might be made at the study midpoint. Neither the subjects nor the study personnel who had contact with them were informed of the study design, nor who was receiving placebo or medication either before or after the crossover. Blinded crossover was facilitated by providing the research pharmacy with each subject's Ham-D–17 score after the first phase of the study. Study material (i.e., capsule containing either sertraline or placebo) was supplied by the research pharmacy on the basis of this score. Subjects in the parent study who responded to placebo in the first phase remained on placebo during the second phase. Those who did not respond to placebo in the 5 weeks of the study were switched to sertraline; these subjects are the focus of our present report.

Sertraline was titrated from a starting dose of 50 mg (1 capsule), with a 50 mg increase on Day 8 (2 capsules), and a final 50 mg increase on Day 15 (3 capsules), to achieve 150 mg. Blinding was preserved by matching the number of capsules taken in the placebo and sertraline conditions. At the point of crossover, each subject, regardless of whether she or he remained on placebo or was crossed over to sertraline, was provided with two pill bottles and was given instructions to cross-titrate from the first pill bottle to the second. Each subject was informed of his or her treatment assignment at the end of the second phase (Week 10) by a staff member not otherwise associated with the study.

In the first phase of the study, 69 subjects had been randomized to placebo, and 45 completed the first 5 weeks of treatment; 30 of these subjects did not respond and therefore were crossed over to sertraline in the second phase of the study; these 30 subjects represented the potential pool for the present report. Of the 30 subjects, 6 were missing at least one EEG, and 2 did not complete medication treatment and therefore were excluded from this analysis. For the 22 remaining subjects, data were analyzed from both EEG recordings, one of which was performed 5 weeks before the other, immediately before the start of medication.

Subjects

Subjects were recruited from outpatient clinics of the UCLA Neuropsychiatric Hospital and through community advertisements. At entry, all subjects had Ham-D–17 scores >168 and met DSM-IV criteria for MDD, diagnosed with the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for DSM-IV.9 All subjects were between 21 and 65 years of age and in good physical health (i.e., free of any medical condition known to affect brain function). Demographics of the sample are shown in Table 1. Subjects were excluded if they received any medication known to significantly affect brain function, if they had a history of suicide attempt, or if they had previously failed to benefit from treatment with sertraline. Subjects were free of any psychotropic medication for at least 2 weeks before enrollment. All experimental procedures were approved by the UCLA Institutional Review Board, and each subject provided written informed consent after receiving a full explanation of the procedures.

TABLE 1. Subject Characteristics
Non-Responders (N=15)Responders (N=7)
Female:male8:74:3
Age, years43.3 (8.9)*32.3 (14.2)*
Previous episodes2.4 (2.1) (N=12)2.3 (0.8) (N=7)
Treatment history (% previously treated)60%*14%*
Baseline Ham-D20.2 (3.9)19.4 (5.9)
Week 5 Ham-D15.5 (4.1)18.3 (5.6)
Week 10 Ham-D15.2 (5.2)6.4 (2.5)

Values are mean (SD), unless otherwise indicated.

* Responder and Non-responder groups differed significantly in age (t[20] = −2.25; p=0.036) and treatment history (χ2=4.02; p=0.045).

TABLE 1. Subject Characteristics
Enlarge table

Clinical Assessment

Clinical symptoms were assessed with the Ham-D–17 at weekly visits. Subjects were classified as medication responders if they showed a ≥50% reduction in Ham-D–17 score over the course of 5 weeks of treatment with sertraline.

EEG Procedures

EEG recording methods have been described previously.5,10,11 We utilized an extended International 10–20 System with an electrode cap of 36 recording electrodes (ElectroCap, Inc.; Eaton, OH) referenced to Pz. Electrode impedances were balanced and under 5kΩ for all channels. The Neuroscan System (Compumedics, Inc.; El Paso, TX) was used to collect up to 20 minutes of EEG data while subjects rested in the eyes-closed, maximally-alert state in a sound-attenuated room with subdued lighting. Patients were frequently and vigorously realerted at the first sign of drowsiness. EEG data were digitized on line at 250 samples/channel/sec., with a high-frequency filter of 70 Hz and a low-frequency filter of 0.3 Hz, as well as a notch filter at 60 Hz. Only EEG that was free of eye movement, drowsiness, excess muscle, or other artifacts was considered for further processing; an EEG technologist selected for processing the first 10 to 30 2-second epochs of data that met this criteria. Twenty seconds of EEG has been shown to be sufficient for characterizing clinically-relevant differences in depression.10,1214 Segments of the recording likely to represent artifact were identified, based upon voltage step gradient, absolute values of difference within the epoch, or persistent low activity. A semi-automated interactive process was used to remove all epochs containing eye movement-, muscle-, or movement-related artifacts, or amplifier drift. Before further processing, a second EEG technologist, blinded to subject identity and treatment condition, independently confirmed that only artifact-free data were selected. EEGs for which 20 seconds of artifact-free data could not be obtained were discarded from further processing. The average number of epochs selected per EEG recording was 17.6 (standard deviation [SD]: 5.5).

LORETA Analysis

The LORETA-KEY package (http://www.unizh.ch/keyinst/loreta)15 was used to compute theta-band (4–7 Hz) current density for each subject. Our LORETA methods have been described previously5 and are summarized here. A cross-spectral matrix was used to compute current density in each of 2,394 cortical voxels. For each subject, theta-band current density was normalized to a total power of 1.0.

Based on previous studies in our lab and others, a region of interest (ROI) was created for the rACC (Figure 1). The rACC ROI consisted of 14 voxels that had previously been shown to differentiate medication responders from non-responders.2,5 All 14 voxels were averaged together to create a single current density value, which was log-transformed to more closely approximate a normal distribution.

FIGURE 1. Sagittal View of the 14-Voxel rACC Region of Interest

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were carried out using Stata 12 (StataCorp LP; College Station, TX). To examine the differences in rACC theta-current density between medication responders and non-responders, we conducted multilevel mixed-effects regression analysis, with rACC theta-current density as the dependent variable. The regression included the factors of Time (2 levels: immediately before or 5 weeks before medication treatment) and Response (2 levels: responders or non-responders), as well as their interaction, all modeled as fixed effects. Because age and treatment history were found to be associated with response in this cohort (see Subject Demographics section), and because antidepressant treatment history has been shown to affect neural activity measured from frontal EEG electrodes,16 the multilevel regression controlled for the factor of Treatment History (2 levels: previous treatment or no previous treatment), as well as the interaction of Treatment History × Time, both as fixed effects. Age was included as a covariate.

Follow-up linear regressions examined rACC activity and treatment history as predictors of percent change in Ham-D–17 score over the course of medication treatment. We employed separate models, to assess rACC activity at baseline and rACC activity at Week 5 as predictors of symptom improvement.

Results

Subject Demographics

Of the 22 subjects, 7 responded and 15 did not respond to medication (Table 1). Responders did not differ significantly from non-responders with respect to gender, baseline, or Week 5 Ham-D–17 scores, or number of previous depressive episodes. Responders were significantly younger than non-responders (t[20] = −2.25; p=0.036). Of non-responders, 60% had previously been treated with antidepressants, whereas only 14% of responders had been previously treated; subjects with previous antidepressant exposure (“treatment-experienced”) were significantly overrepresented in the non-responder group (χ2=4.02; p=0.045).

Multilevel, Mixed-Effects Regression

As hypothesized, the multilevel, mixed-effects regression on rACC activity found a significant Response × Time interaction, indicating that the relationship between response and rACC activity level depended on the time of the rACC assessment (χ2=6.33; p=0.012). The difference in rACC theta-current density between responders and non-responders was greater at the time-point immediately before, as compared with 5 weeks before medication onset (Figure 2). Other significant effects included a main effect of Response (χ2=5.58; p=0.018) and a Treatment History × Time interaction (χ2=11.96; p=0.001). As shown in Figure 3, the difference in rACC activity was greater between treatment-naïve versus treatment-experienced subjects at the rACC time-point immediately before medication (Figure 3). There was no significant main effect of Time or Treatment History.

FIGURE 2. rACC Theta-Current Density for Medication Responders and Non-Responders 5 Weeks Before, Versus Immediately Before Medication
FIGURE 3. Log rACC Theta-Current Density for Response Groups × Treatment History

Linear Regression

Follow-up linear-regression analyses examined rACC theta-current density as a predictor of clinical improvement, assessed as percent change in Ham-D–17 during sertraline treatment. Baseline rACC theta-current density and rACC theta current-density measured at Week 5 were evaluated in separate models. Each model included log-transformed rACC theta-current density, Treatment History, and the rACC × Treatment History interaction as predictors, with age as a covariate. The model examining baseline rACC did not demonstrate a significant interaction or a significant main effect of theta-current density or treatment history. At Week 5, however, there was a significant interaction between rACC activity and treatment history (b=1.495; standard error [SE]=0.586; t=2.55; p=0.021), indicating a different relationship between rACC and clinical improvement for subjects with versus those without previous treatment. That is, the slope of “change in Ham-D–17” on rACC theta-current density was significantly different for each level of treatment history; slope = −0.670 for treatment-naïve subjects, as compared with +0.825 for treatment-experienced subjects (Figure 4). Regarding other factors in the model, the main effect of treatment history was significant (b=1.493; SE=0.526; t=2.84; p=0.011), and the main effect of rACC approached the threshold for significance (b = −0.70; SE=0.327; t = −2.05; p=0.057).

FIGURE 4. Slopes by Treatment History Using rACC Activity Measured Immediately Before Medication Treatment to Predict Degree of Response

The relationship between rACC activity and degree of response differed significantly between treatment-naive and treatment-experienced subjects. Shaded regions indicate standard error.

Discussion

These results demonstrate that the relationship between rACC activity level and clinical response to antidepressant medication was influenced by the time of the rACC assessment. The relationship between rACC activity and response was stronger when rACC level was measured just before the beginning of medication, as compared with 5 weeks before. Theta-current source density in the rACC region therefore may capture a state-related aspect of brain functioning that is associated with subsequent response to medication.

To our knowledge, this is the first report on rACC activity at multiple time-points before medication treatment. Implicit in our results is that the state of responsiveness reflected in rACC theta may change in an individual over time within a single episode of MDD. Many factors could have contributed to changes in rACC activity in these subjects, including natural history, life events, endogenous biological processes, and treatment with placebo. Although subjects in the present study had failed to respond clinically to an initial 5 weeks of placebo treatment, we do not know to what extent placebo treatment may have influenced brain changes over that time. Placebo treatment in the clinical-trial setting is a multifaceted intervention that involves regular visits to a healthcare setting, ongoing contact with the treating physician and other healthcare personnel, as well as daily ingestion of the placebo pill. Any or all of these aspects may have contributed to increased expectations of symptom improvement. Previous work has shown that changes in prefrontal theta cordance during a 1-week placebo lead-in are related to clinical outcomes in MDD during subsequent treatment with serotonin reuptake-inhibitor antidepressant medications.11,17,18 Of note, theta activity measured from prefrontal surface electrodes has been linked to activity in ACC as measured using magnetoencephalography (MEG)19,20 and positron emission tomography (PET).21 Although imaging data on the expectation of symptom improvement in MDD has yet to be fully reported,22 fMRI imaging in an experimental pain paradigm found anticipation of analgesia associated with increases in rACC activity.23 A specific role of patients’ expectations in the relationship between rACC activity and clinical response to treatment for MDD has yet to be elucidated.

Our findings suggest that future work should account for the potential effects of previous antidepressant treatment on the relationship between rACC activity and clinical response. In the present results, whereas higher rACC activity was associated with greater improvement among subjects who had never before received antidepressant treatment, this pattern was not observed in antidepressant-experienced subjects. We previously reported that treatment history, independent of illness severity as assessed using baseline Ham-D score or number of previous episodes, influenced prefrontal brain changes (EEG) observed during treatment.16 Previous exposure to antidepressant medication may influence brain functional changes observed during treatment with medication or placebo caused by psychological conditioning (e.g., pharmaco-conditioning)24 or physiological adaptation. Also, patients who have undergone more antidepressant medication treatment may show differences in ACC metabolism,25 levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF),26 and serotonergic function.27,28

The neurobiologic mechanisms by which elevated rACC activity relates to improved treatment response are not well elucidated, although fronto-limbic neuroanatomy and function suggest several possible routes of influence. The rACC is an important area in the limbic–cortical dysregulation model of MDD;29 it has bilateral reciprocal connections with the amygdala, as well as a broad extent of the prefrontal cortex.30,31 The rACC processes emotion and attention,32 and abnormal activity may affect the brain’s ability to regulate these processes. During the resolution of emotional conflict, increased rACC activity inhibits the amygdala, as well as the autonomic stress response.33 Increased activation of the rACC in MDD is also associated with inhibition of focusing on negative words.34 Also, the neurotransmitter serotonin may offer insight into how elevated rACC activity relates to treatment response, as it plays an important role in proper rACC functioning,3537 as well as the treatment of depression. Elevated rACC activity may potentiate the ability of medication or other treatments to alter dysfunctional patterns of activity in MDD.

Results of this study should be interpreted with consideration of the following points: First, it is important to keep in mind that the main finding, supported by the statistically significant Response × Time interaction, was observed after controlling for several covariates. Second, the study has a limited number of subjects, who may represent a select population, because subjects included in this analysis had all failed to respond to a 5-week course of double-blinded treatment with placebo before being crossed over to medication. Thus, these subjects may be considered treatment-refractory to placebo. Third, treatment with medication lasted only 5 weeks. This course of treatment may not have been adequate to determine full response to medication, especially given that sertraline was not titrated to the full dose (150 mg.) until the beginning of Week 3. Both previous failure on placebo and inadequate duration of treatment at the full dose of sertraline may explain the low response rate (32%) that was observed after the crossover. More subjects might have responded with a longer duration of treatment. Despite these limitations, the predictive relationship between the timing of the rACC measure and medication response still holds.

Conclusion And Future Directions

Results of this study suggest that elevated rACC activity may represent a “state” marker of responsiveness to antidepressant treatment. If rACC activity represents a state marker that varies over the course of a depressive episode, it is possible that the timing with which treatment is introduced could be important for clinical response. One research approach would be to prospectively initiate treatment coincident with high versus low rACC activity to determine whether this improves clinical outcome. Another question is whether specific interventions or practices could lead to state changes (e.g., increases) in rACC activity, that would “prime” the brain for better response to treatment. For example, cognitive-behavioral therapy38 and mindfulness meditation, an aspect of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy,39 both have been shown to activate the rACC. Further prospective research is required to replicate results and determine a possible causal role for elevated rACC activity in antidepressant response that would have implications for improving treatment strategies in MDD.

Laboratory of Brain, Behavior, and Pharmacology, and the UCLA Depression Research & Clinic Program, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior at UCLA; Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, CA.
Send correspondence to: Aimee M. Hunter, Ph.D.; e-mail:

Drs. Hunter and Korb contributed equally to the manuscript.

Portions of this manuscript were submitted by Alexander S. Korb in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Neuroscience at the University of California Los Angeles.

This research was supported by grant R01AT002479 from the National Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine. Support was also received from Pfizer, Inc., and Eli Lilly and Company.

References

1 Mayberg HS, Brannan SK, Mahurin RK, et al.: Cingulate function in depression: a potential predictor of treatment response. Neuroreport 1997; 8:1057–1061Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Pizzagalli D, Pascual-Marqui RD, Nitschke JB, et al.: Anterior cingulate activity as a predictor of degree of treatment response in major depression: evidence from brain electrical tomography analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:405–415Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 Saxena S, Brody AL, Ho ML, et al.: Differential brain metabolic predictors of response to paroxetine in obsessive-compulsive disorder versus major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:522–532Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Mulert C, Juckel G, Brunnmeier M, et al.: Rostral anterior cingulate cortex activity in the theta band predicts response to antidepressive medication. Clin EEG Neurosci 2007; 38:78–81Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Korb AS, Hunter AM, Cook IA, et al.: Rostral anterior cingulate cortex theta current density and response to antidepressants and placebo in major depression. Clin Neurophysiol 2009; 120:1313–1319Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Pizzagalli DA: Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011; 36:183–206Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Korb AS, Hunter AM, Cook IA, et al.: Rostral anterior cingulate cortex activity and early symptom improvement during treatment for major depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res 2011; 192:188–194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, et al.: The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 1998; 59(Suppl 20):22–33, quiz 34–57MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Cook IA, Leuchter AF, Morgan M, et al.: Early changes in prefrontal activity characterize clinical responders to antidepressants. Neuropsychopharmacology 2002; 27:120–131Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Hunter AM, Leuchter AF, Morgan ML, et al.: Changes in brain function (quantitative EEG cordance) during placebo lead-in and treatment outcomes in clinical trials for major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1426–1432Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Mientus S, Gallinat J, Wuebben Y, et al.: Cortical hypoactivation during resting EEG in schizophrenics but not in depressives and schizotypal subjects as revealed by low-resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA). Psychiatry Res 2002; 116:95–111Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Bares M, Brunovsky M, Kopecek M, et al.: Changes in QEEG prefrontal cordance as a predictor of response to antidepressants in patients with treatment-resistant depressive disorder: a pilot study. J Psychiatr Res 2007; 41:319–325Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Korb AS, Cook IA, Hunter AM, et al.: Brain electrical source differences between depressed subjects and healthy controls. Brain Topogr 2008; 21:138–146Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Pascual-Marqui RD, Lehmann D, Koenig T, et al.: Low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) functional imaging in acute, neuroleptic-naive, first-episode, productive schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1999; 90:169–179Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Hunter AM, Cook IA, Leuchter AF: Does prior antidepressant treatment of major depression impact brain function during current treatment? Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2012; 22:711–720.Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17 Hunter AM, Leuchter AF, Morgan ML, et al.: Neurophysiologic correlates of side effects in normal subjects randomized to venlafaxine or placebo. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005; 30:792–799Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Hunter AM, Ravikumar S, Cook IA, et al.: Brain functional changes during placebo lead-in and changes in specific symptoms during pharmacotherapy for major depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009; 119:266–273Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Asada H, Fukuda Y, Tsunoda S, et al.: Frontal midline theta rhythms reflect alternative activation of prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in humans. Neurosci Lett 1999; 274:29–32Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Ishii R, Shinosaki K, Ukai S, et al.: Medial prefrontal cortex generates frontal midline theta rhythm. Neuroreport 1999; 10:675–679Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Pizzagalli DA, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ: Coupling of theta activity and glucose metabolism in the human rostral anterior cingulate cortex: an EEG/PET study of normal and depressed subjects. Psychophysiology 2003; 40:939–949Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Rutherford BR, Peterson BS, Roose SP: Patient expectancy of improvement may influence neuroimaging studies of Major Depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/cgi/eletters/67/11/1128Google Scholar

23 Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, et al.: Placebo-induced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science 2004; 303:1162–1167Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Ader R: The role of conditioning in pharmacotherapy, in The Placebo Effect. Edited by Harrington A. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press, 1997Google Scholar

25 Brody AL, Saxena S, Stoessel P, et al.: Regional brain metabolic changes in patients with major depression treated with either paroxetine or interpersonal therapy: preliminary findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:631–640Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Shimizu E, Hashimoto K, Okamura N, et al.: Alterations of serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in depressed patients with or without antidepressants. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54:70–75Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Messa C, Colombo C, Moresco RM, et al.: 5-HT(2A) receptor binding is reduced in drug-naive and unchanged in SSRI-responder depressed patients compared to healthy controls: a PET study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2003; 167:72–78Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Parsey RV, Hastings RS, Oquendo MA, et al.: Lower serotonin transporter binding potential in the human brain during major depressive episodes. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:52–58Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Mayberg HS: Modulating dysfunctional limbic-cortical circuits in depression: towards development of brain-based algorithms for diagnosis and optimised treatment. Br Med Bull 2003; 65:193–207Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Cohen MX, Heller AS, Ranganath C: Functional connectivity with anterior cingulate and orbitofrontal cortices during decision-making. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2005; 23:61–70Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Nieuwenhuys R, Voogd J, van Huijzen C: The Human Central Nervous System, 4th Edition. Berlin, Germany, Springer, 2007Google Scholar

32 Lane RD, Reiman EM, Axelrod B, et al.: Neural correlates of levels of emotional awareness: evidence of an interaction between emotion and attention in the anterior cingulate cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 1998; 10:525–535Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33 Etkin A, Egner T, Peraza DM, et al.: Resolving emotional conflict: a role for the rostral anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron 2006; 51:871–882Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34 Eugène F, Joormann J, Cooney RE, et al.: Neural correlates of inhibitory deficits in depression. Psychiatry Res 2010; 181:30–35Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

35 Morris JS, Smith KA, Cowen PJ, et al.: Covariation of activity in habenula and dorsal raphé nuclei following tryptophan depletion. Neuroimage 1999; 10:163–172Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Sargent PA, Kjaer KH, Bench CJ, et al.: Brain serotonin1A receptor binding measured by positron emission tomography with [11C]WAY-100635: effects of depression and antidepressant treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:174–180Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Varnäs K, Halldin C, Hall H: Autoradiographic distribution of serotonin transporters and receptor subtypes in human brain. Hum Brain Mapp 2004; 22:246–260Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, et al.: Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive-behavior therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:34–41Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Chiesa A, Serretti A: Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for psychiatric disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 2011; 187:441–453Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar