The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Special ArticlesFull Access

The Dialectic Between Empathy and Violence: An Opportunity for Intervention?

Abstract

The authors provide a comprehensive review of the neurobiology of empathy and compare this with the neurobiology of psychopathic predatory violence—the most extreme deficit of empathy. This suggests that the specific areas of the prefrontal cortex and limbic system, which have been associated with violent behavior, also appear to subserve the capacity for empathy. Damage to these regions may result in the emergence of aggression, but not of empathy, suggesting a structurally inverse relationship between the two. The authors examine the evidence for a dialectic between empathy and predatory violence and explore the implications for early interventions with empathy training in treatment-resistant psychopathy.

The search for a violence center within the brain has preoccupied anatomists and behaviorists for centuries. In the recent past, deficits in specific areas of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the limbic system have been most consistently associated with violent behavior.1 Similar regions of the brain also subserve the capacity for empathy. Damage to these regions may result in the emergence of aggression but not empathy, suggesting an inverse relationship between empathy and violence. This review of the neurobiology of empathy compared with the extreme deficit of empathy (i.e., psychopathic predatory violence [PV]) points to the possibility that early interventions with empathy training may lessen psychopathy resistance to treatment.

Defining Empathy

Empathy, or “feeling as another does,”2 functionally comprises four dimensions. Empathy is (a) an affective state that is (b) isomorphic to another person’s affective state, (c) is elicited by observing or imagining another person’s affective state, and (d) is experienced while remaining cognizant that the other person’s affective state is the source of one’s own affective state.3,4 The development of empathy is preceded by, and emerges from, more elementary functions such as brainstem-mediated mimicry, which is present at birth, and mirror-neuron–mediated emotional resonance, which emerges in the very first months of life.5 Both of these functions induce physiological changes, such as facial grimacing or pupillary dilation, in response to the expressions, vocalizations, postures, and movements of another person.6 During the second year of life, at the same time as frontally mediated self/other cognitive awareness begins to develop, this capacity to send and respond to limbic-modulated emotional signals evolves into more mature forms of empathy. Our capacity to distinguish whether the source of an affective experience is triggered by another or lies within ourselves is a key characteristic of empathy7 and is part of a broader capacity for perspective taking. Although compassion (i.e., sympathy or empathic concern) also induces affective changes in the observer, empathy denotes that the observer’s emotions reflect affective sharing (“feeling as” the other person), whereas compassion denotes that the observer’s emotions are inherently other oriented (“feeling for” the other person).8 Empathic perspective taking also partially differs from mentalizing and theory-of-mind functions, which involve taking another person's perspective and attributing to them particular cognitive states, in that it is more involved in attributing emotional states.9

The Neurobiology of Empathy

The PFC is subdivided into five frontal-subcortical regions, two of which have most consistently been implicated in violent and empathic behavior: the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit, which connects pathways that modulate executive functions, including the ability to plan, problem solve, sequence events, and adaptively change cognitive and behavioral sets; and the orbitofrontal circuit, which connects frontal monitoring pathways to the limbic system and governs appropriate responses to social cues and interpersonal sensitivity.10 Corticolimbic networks subserving distinct social functions can be further divided into three partially dissociable networks: perceptual, subserving awareness/understanding of others’ socioemotional behavior (lateral orbitofrontal cortex [OFC], ventrolateral temporal pole, fusiform gyrus, superior temporal sulcus); reward/affiliation, subserving socioemotional responsiveness/detachment (dorsomedial temporal pole, rostral anterior cingulate cortex, subgenual anterior cingulate cortex, ventromedial PFC [vmPFC], entorhinal cortex, parahippocampal cortex, ventromedial striatum); and pain/aversion, subserving threat detection and approach-avoidance behaviors (caudal anterior cingulate cortex, insula, somatosensory operculum, ventrolateral striatum).11 The limbic system includes the amygdala, which attributes emotional valence to memories; the hypothalamus, which receives information about the internal state of the body and orchestrates endocrine/hormonal responses through its control of the pituitary gland; and the cingulate gyrus, which is involved in autonomic regulatory functions such as heart rate and blood pressure.1

The affective and cognitive components of empathy are dissociable, as indicated by neurological12,13 and functional14,15 studies as well as by their different developmental trajectories.16,17 Mature empathic sensitivity depends on the functional integration of these components, expressed via emotional regulation and attachment behaviors, which typically develop in tandem.

The affective component of empathy relies on a neural resonance system by which an observer engages motor intention,18 sensory experience,19 and visceral state20 neural mechanisms, which overlap with those that the individual would engage if he or she were directly experiencing a given internal state. The cognitive component of empathy engages the ability to represent affective states outside of a perceiver's present experience to include anticipated experiences or the experiences of another (self-projection).21,22 Brain regions most typically associated with affective empathy include the inferior parietal lobule, anterior insula, posterior superior temporal sulcus, and anterior cingulate cortex. Cognitive empathy engages a system of midline and superior temporal structures broadly involved in “self-projection” and mentalizing. These include the temporoparietal junction, temporal poles, medial PFC, posterior cingulate cortex, and precuneus.4

Although these brain regions that subserve affective and cognitive components constitute a complex distributed and recursively interconnected network, further activating autonomic and neuroendocrine processes implicated in social behaviors and emotional states,8 recent studies have begun to detect temporal dynamics within the process of empathic experience that indicate brain activity associated with affective sharing comes online earlier than the mentalizing-related activity.23

Developmental shifts take place within this network, which allow for the transition from emotional arousal and self-distress to more mature empathic responsiveness.24 As a child matures from 6 to 11 years old, the self/other awareness circuit becomes more selectively responsive to perspective-taking situations that require inferring the mental states of others.25 At the same time, the development of affective processing from childhood to adulthood is accompanied by reduced activity within the brainstem and limbic affective systems and by the increased involvement of the PFC.26 In response to others’ distress, younger children recruit the amygdala, medial OFC, and posterior portion of the insula more so than adults.27 As children mature, the activity of the medial OFC, which is involved in regulating motor and visceral responses, decreases and the activity of the lateral OFC, which is involved in executive control of emotion reactivity, increases.28 This pattern of developmental change is indicative of a gradual shift from the monitoring of somatovisceral responses in young children to a more cognitive, evaluative level, which is associated with executive control of emotions in adults.29 As cortical executive functions mature through childhood and adolescence, inhibitory capacities and attentional control strengthen, allowing for more fine-tuned emotional regulation. Activation of these prefrontal functions reduces amygdala and autonomic reactivity.30 Overall, as children mature, there is a progressive shift from more limbic to more frontal activation. Inhibitory control and emotional regulation are linked to the ventral and dorsal aspects of the PFC and to the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, both through their reciprocal connections with limbic areas.31 Emotional regulation is fundamental for the capacity to experience empathy rather than personal distress.27 Well-regulated children are more prone to empathy, regardless of their emotional reactivity, because they have learned, in part through the support of their caregivers, to modulate their negative vicarious emotions to maintain an optimal level of emotional arousal. By contrast, children who are unable to regulate their emotions tend to be low in empathy and to become overwhelmed by their negative emotions when witnessing another in distress.16

Empathy evolves in the service of attachment for self-preservation. Empathy enhances survival by bonding individuals, especially mother and infant, thereby increasing defenses against predators.32 By reducing personal distress and avoidance behaviors, secure attachment facilitates affect regulation, in turn increasing empathic behaviors.16 Children with secure attachment are more empathic toward others, regardless of relatedness.33 Conversely, lack of secure attachment increases avoidant behavior, emotional distress, and lack of empathy.34 The degree of empathic responsiveness directly correlates with approach-avoidance behaviors, which directly modulate attachment. Approach-avoidance behaviors are hormonally modulated.35 In humans, the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and oxytocin are particularly relevant. The HPA axis is functional at birth and matures rapidly during the early years, lessening emotional lability and increasing self-control.36 This process is strictly linked to the presence and responsiveness of an attachment figure in the child’s life, which specifically triggers up/down HPA regulation,37 with gender- and parental-status differences in neurohemodynamic brain responses to infants. Female, but not male, individuals exhibit regulatory changes in response to infant stimuli, with mothers showing greater modulation in response to crying and nonmothers in response to laughing.38 The social modulation of physiological stress responses continues to influence HPA activity in adults and provides a buffer against stress.39 The extent of this shift is also affected by individual predisposition toward autonomic arousal, emotional reactivity, and strength of executive functions. Oxytocin, which is released in the context of supportive relationships, has specific modulatory effects on the HPA axis. By downregulating the HPA axis, it induces increased tolerance to stressful stimuli by reducing pain sensitivity, fear/anxiety, and defensive avoidance, thereby enabling greater trust, attachment, and empathy.40 Both secure attachment and higher degrees of empathic capacity hormonally activate the brain reward system, which, in turn, sustains attachment and nurturance. Because empathy and attachment are linked to the same hormonal events, they are both behaviorally and physiologically interdependent. The hormonal shifts of pregnancy predispose the brain reward system to form mother-infant bonds at birth,41 while securely attached mother-child interactions increase the production of oxytocin, activating the brain reward regions.42 Even when empathic behaviors are extended to nonkin, these behaviors activate the reward system, inducing feelings of well-being.43 Thus, empathic behaviors are physiologically rewarding, if not addictive.44

Aggression, Violence, and the Lack of Empathy

Aggression is “an intentional act that inflicts bodily or mental harm on others,”45 which is legitimate in certain contexts (e.g., self-defense) and may occur without bodily damage (e.g., verbal aggression). By contrast, violence is “an aggressive act that causes physical injury” and is the subset of aggression “characterized by the unwarranted infliction of physical harm.”45(p. 2) This review focuses on interpersonal violence, excluding socially sanctioned types of violence (e.g., sports), warfare, totalitarian regimes, and organized crime (e.g., gangs, terrorism, and the Mafia). The literature on interpersonal violence distinguishes two main types of violence: impulsive violence (IV; “expressive,” “affective,” or “reactive”), which is characterized by rage or other intense emotions, and predatory violence (PV; “calculated/instrumental,” “callous-unemotional” [CU], or “proactive”).46,47 Perpetrators who exhibit IV tend to target individuals with whom they have a connection: spouses, family members, coworkers, and schoolmates. Individuals who exhibit PV may target proximity victims, but they more often perpetrate PV against unknown others. We will make reference to IV only to the extent that it speaks to the understanding of PV. Although overt sexual violence is more present in IV, sexual arousal without overt sexual behavior is thought to be more present in PV, possibly as a motivating factor for the violent act. Acts without direct sexual contact may be arousing or otherwise intensely stimulating to the perpetrator. However, in the absence of scientific methods or criteria to distinguish impulsive versus predatory sexual violence, we will not consider the potential role of sexual violence in PV in this review.

PV may closely represent the inverse of empathy. Cold-blooded, purposeful PV is the hallmark of psychopathy.4850 Although they are not equivalent concepts, there is a substantial overlap between psychopathy and PV. Although not all psychopathic domains include opportunism and instrumentalism,51 the literature on psychopathy consistently identifies both empathic deficits and predatory behaviors as the core signature of a psychopathic personality.52 PV has also been found to correlate with an individual’s total score on the Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Youth Version and interpersonal features of psychopathy.50 The psychopathic behavior of PV is found in individuals who present with a dysfunction in either experiencing or sharing feelings with others, which is associated with deficits in affective empathy.53,54 By contrast, individuals who exhibit IV tend to display disinhibition and intense emotions and more typically retain a capacity for empathy and remorse.55 Unfortunately, the systematic investigation of psychopathy has been pursued only very recently, notwithstanding the descriptive reports of behavioral presentations such as the numerous editions of Cleckley’s56Mask of Sanity.

The Neurobiology of PV

Lesion and Genetic Correlates of PV

Lesion and brain-imaging studies have indicated that functional disruption of the PFC and limbic system (and with it, the HPA axis) can lead to the emergence of aggression and violence in a multitude of different conditions. However, lesion studies have failed to identify a specific violence center and have elicited a host of seemingly contradictory findings. Damage to the PFC can induce psychopathic traits such as lack of empathy and blunted emotions,57 impaired moral judgment,58 and impaired perspective taking with increased egocentrism and rigidity10,59; however, damage to the PFC more frequently induces emotional dysregulation, impulsivity, and poor planning.57,60 Damage to the amygdala and hippocampus can induce increased avoidance behaviors61 but can also increase startle reflex62 and fear response.63 Although these seemingly contradictory results are attributable to a range of anatomical and technological limitations intrinsic to lesion studies, which do not allow for fine neurobehavioral distinctions, it is an empirical fact that patients with acquired lesions in the vmPFC, OFC, and amygdalae and related limbic structures do not typically exhibit PV, although they may exhibit other forms of violence.64 One reason for this is that these individuals may retain other functions that allow them to reflect and follow remembered rules, or they may retain important components of empathy. The link between empathy and PV is not linear (all or none). Rather, the main structures and networks thought to participate in the dialectic between empathy and PV function in synchrony with and are finely modulated by input from other structures, often in recursive manners. Indeed, even some individuals with psychopathic behavior may express empathy for selected groups or animals or in specific situations.65,66

Genetic studies have suggested a predisposition to violence and aggression. Sibling and twin studies first provided evidence of familial aggregation of criminal activity and suggested that genetic factors account for approximately 40%−50% of the variance in transmission.67 Genetic studies have supported a genetic predisposition only when including a broad range of antisocial behaviors68 but have failed to provide evidence for a specific violence gene. The XYY chromosome theory, once linked to gender differences in violence, was not supported by further findings.45 Although individual differences in monoamine oxidase-A (MAO-A) genetic alleles were once regarded as promising arenas for identifying a violence gene, studies have indicated that different versions of the gene are found in different individuals, that the distribution of these different versions differs from one ethnic group to another (making cultural-ethnic factors difficult to control),69 and that MAO-A alleles have been associated only with broadly defined antisocial behaviors. An interaction between a history of childhood maltreatment and MAO-A status has been supported in some studies,70 and other genes (catechol-O-methyltransferase, dopamine transporter 1, dopamine receptor genes DRD2 and DRD4, and serotonin transporter polymorphism) are being investigated as potentially linked to antisocial behavior.71 Although multiple genes may interact to predispose individuals to violent behavior, the overall heritability rate of approximately 50% is similar to that for many other behaviors, indicating an equally important role of environmental factors.72 Subsequent longitudinal studies of combined low-activity–inducing MAO-A variations and exposure to early traumatic life events have shown a correlation only with broadly defined antisocial behaviors.70,73,74 There are also other problems with the view that MAO-A variations have a significant role in PV. Regardless of the allele type, significant levels of antisocial behaviors have been found in adults having no history of abuse.70 Furthermore, the assumption that the level of allele activity directly affects the brain70 has not turned out to be true in vivo, undercutting the view that the low-activity variant of the “warrior gene” actually results in low enzyme activity in the brain.75 Finally, more than one in three men carry the relevant genetic variant, although the vast majority of them do not commit violent crimes. Thus, at most, this variant may produce a small increase in the risk for antisocial behavior among men with a history of abuse.76 These studies indicate that violence is a multifactorial phenomenon, better understood within a framework of gene-environment interactions, with genetic differences creating a susceptibility to particular environmental risk factors. This paradigm can indicate a trend within a particular population but cannot make specific predictions at the individual level, where factors other than genetics play an equally important role. These studies also have not addressed the important distinction between IV and PV. The systematic investigation of PV has only been possible in recent years with the emergence of sophisticated neuroimaging technologies.

Structural and Functional Correlates of PV

The systematic investigation of the neurobiology of PV psychopathy was recently spearheaded by British neuroscientist Adrian Raine and his colleagues through a series of seminal articles, which indicate cognitive- and affective/emotional processing deficits in psychopathy associated with abnormal brain structure and function, particularly in the amygdala, OFC, and vmPFC, as well as possibly diminished cortisol levels.46 Early brain-imaging studies were based on the broad clinical diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder,48,77 without distinguishing between IV and PV. Because psychopathy was considered a subtype of antisocial behavior, these early studies failed to identify differences in their underlying deficits.55,78,79

In recent studies, PV was found to correlate with normal or increased prefrontal activity, increased intrafrontal connectivity, and reduced PFC-limbic/paralimbic functional connectivity, associated with inflexible behavior in PV psychopathy.8082 PV was also shown to correlate with hypometabolism in limbic structures (amygdala/hippocampal formation, parahippocampal gyrus, ventral striatum, and anterior/posterior cingulate gyri),46,83 which correlates with shallow affect and lack of affective empathy; and, less frequently, hypermetabolism,82 which is associated with aggressive impulses. Additional findings include volumetric changes, with increased callosal white matter volume and length, and increased functional interhemispheric connectivity, correlating mainly with deficient affect,84 and deficits in the OFC, vmPFC, and cingulate cortex associated with impaired moral judgment. Conversely, IV has been found to relatively consistently correlate with PFC metabolic reduction, limbic hyperactivity, and lower prefrontal/subcortical ratios, indicating deficits in frontal regulation of limbic/paralimbic aggressive impulses.82,85 Consistently, individuals with psychopathy have been found to suffer from emotional empathy deficit disorder (the capacity to experience or share emotions) subserved by the limbic system17,86 but have retained the capacity to understand the feelings of others (cognitive empathy) subserved by the frontal circuits.87,88

Additional studies, without differentiating PV and IV, found decreased regional gray matter volume in the limbic/paralimbic regions, including the OFC, bilateral temporal poles, and posterior cingulate cortex,8991 and lower amygdala volume associated with higher levels of aggression and psychopathic features from childhood to adulthood.92,93 Structural abnormalities have been found in psychopathic individuals with criminal convictions (termed “unsuccessful”) relative to those without convictions (termed “successful”). Unsuccessful psychopathic individuals show increased anterior hippocampal volume asymmetry (right greater than left)94 and reduced PFC gray matter volume.95 These findings have been interpreted as correlating with emotional dysregulation and reduced fear conditioning in unsuccessful psychopathic individuals, who are consequently less sensitive to environmental cues of danger and capture. The significance of these findings will depend on whether they can be shown to consistently correlate with specific aspects of cognitive or affective processing in individuals who exhibit specific types of violence.

Neurochemical and Hormonal Correlates of PV

Motivated by the hope for neuropharmacological interventions, the neurochemistry of violence has received considerable attention. Testosterone has received particular attention, because the incidence of violence is much higher in male individuals and because involvement in crime usually rises in the early to mid-teens (when testosterone levels rise). A testosterone-aggression link has been shown in animal studies but has not been shown convincingly in humans.96 For example, although incarcerated violent criminals have higher testosterone levels,97 elevated testosterone levels may be an effect of violence, rather than a cause. It has been hypothesized that features of psychopathy such as hyporesponsivity to stressors, reduced fear, reduced sensitivity to punishment, and enhanced sensitivity to reward may result from the combined effect of reduced cortisol levels and increased testosterone levels.98,99 The interplay between altered hormonal peripheral steroid hormones such as cortisol and the insula, anterior cingulate cortex, and amygdala has been found to be associated with diminished sensitivity to stress and increased callousness.100 One study found that testosterone administration led to decreased sensitivity to punishment and increased sensitivity to reward.101,102 These findings suggest a link between cortisol-testosterone balance and callousness, potentially predisposing to psychopathy.103 Although neurotransmitters have been extensively investigated, studies suggest a link between serotonin and norepinephrine with impulsivity and with IV but not PV.104 In addition, despite a growing literature on the dopaminergic and vasopressinergic systems,99 little is known about the functional interaction of neurotransmitters with frontal and other neuroanatomical structures.104

Neuropsychological Deficits in PV

Although neuropsychological deficits in attention, language, and executive functioning have been found in the population who exhibits PV, their specificity is questionable. Disturbances of attention and executive functioning are present in many neurological disorders, both neurodevelopmental and acquired. Thus, these neuropsychological abnormalities may not be pathognomic of psychopathy. There is inconsistent evidence of superior capacity for selective or focused attention paired with diminished capacity for attention shifting to secondary information while engaged in goal-directed behavior (response modulation)105 and of reduced capacity for divided attention (multitasking).106 Investigations of complex executive functions have identified deficits in orbitofrontal-mediated speeded binary decision making (as assessed by the go/no-go test) and resistance to interference (as assessed by the Stroop test), but not in dorsolateral-mediated skills in set shifting, flexibility, and responsiveness to feedback (as assessed by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test or the Trail Making Tests).107,108 Orbitofrontal-mediated risk-taking tasks (e.g., the Iowa gambling task) have demonstrated consistent deficits,109 although group differences secondary to reduced anxiety110 or poor attention,111 rather than psychopathy, could not be ruled out. Because psychopathic individuals typically express, in language, emotions that they do not feel, language processing has also been investigated. Studies have found a lack of facilitation on lexical-decision tasks for affective, relative to neutral, words112; significantly reduced affective, but not semantic, priming107; and reduced understanding of the emotional valence of metaphors despite literal understanding.113 These findings, however, may not indicate specific language deficits. Alternatively, they may be explained by the better-established findings of impaired emotional processing in psychopathy.

Heterogeneity of neuropsychological findings may be partly attributable to the existence of subgroups of psychopathic individuals. For example, unsuccessful (convicted) psychopathic individuals show executive functioning deficits, whereas successful (nonconvicted) ones do not. The latter even outperformed control participants on executive functioning.114 Thus, better executive functioning may protect a subgroup of psychopathic individuals from being detected and arrested.

Psychophysiological Correlates of PV

Psychopathic individuals are less responsive than nonpsychopathic offenders when anticipating or reacting to unpleasant stimuli, whether measured electrodermally115 or by startle blink.116 Measurements of event-related potentials (ERPs) show mixed results. Some P300 studies, involving a waveform linked to deployment of neural resources to task-relevant information, have been inconsistent.117,118 Studies of other components of ERPs indicate reduced response inhibition,119 reduced affective sensitivity for facial expressions of emotion,120 and abnormal late negativity, maximal over frontocentral regions, with various stimulus-processing and decision-making tasks.117 Reduced event-related negativity (a potential that peaks after an incorrect response in speeded reaction-time paradigms) has been observed in antisocial personality121 and in psychopathic offenders,122 possibly indicating error-detection deficits121 or conflict-monitoring impairments.122 However, these findings have not been replicated in other studies.123

Although overall baseline differences in heart rate or electrodermal arousal have not been found in individuals with psychopathy,115 reduced heart rate reactivity to stress has been found in unsuccessful psychopathic individuals and increased reactivity in successful ones.114 These findings may indicate that autonomic impairments are specific to unsuccessful psychopathic individuals or that different features of psychopathy have distinct etiologies and that only the affective-interpersonal features are associated with abnormal autonomic reactivity.124 If supported by future research, findings of diminished autonomic (especially electrodermal and startle reflex) reactivity to stressful/aversive stimuli in psychopathic individuals would be consistent with theories emphasizing their punishment insensitivity and reduced fear125 in relationship to their affective-interpersonal capacities.126

Neurodevelopmental Origins and CU Traits

When brain damage occurs early in life, psychopathic-like effects are more pronounced, a finding that has been interpreted as supporting the view of psychopathy as a neurodevelopmental deficit.46 Compared with damage acquired in adulthood, damage to the vmPFC before age 16 months has been linked to a significantly higher risk for the development of more severe abusive/criminal behaviors and reduced empathy/remorse,59 because early damage to the vmPFC disrupts moral development.85,127 Children and adolescents with CU traits persistently exhibit disregard for others, lack of empathy, and deficient affect. Such emotional and behavioral dysregulation distinguishes them from other antisocial youth and associates them with psychopathic adults.128 Adolescents with high CU traits are more likely to engage in bullying,129 to exhibit more severe instrumental aggression,128 to be less sensitive to punishment,130 and to expect more positive outcomes in aggressive situations131 than conduct-disordered adolescents without CU traits.

Because of inherent ethical complications, studies on the heritability of CU traits are rare, and better research is needed to understand the genetic-epigenetic interplay.132 A twin study of CU traits found a heritability rate of 42%, which is similar to that found in adults.133 Another study with conduct-disordered children with high CU traits reported a higher rate of 81%.134 Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of children with CU traits show decreased amygdala activation in response to fear and stress,135137 decreased OFC-amygdala functional connectivity in moral judgments,138 and decreased connectivity to the vmPFC, with symptom severity correlating negatively with connection strength.139 Neuroanatomical findings include frontal alterations associated with poor decision making,140,141 striatal structural alterations possibly associated with sensation-seeking and reward-driven behavior,137,141 a thicker temporal cortex (particularly in male individuals),142 and an increased incidence of cavum septum pellucidum (a marker of prenatal limbic and septal neural maldevelopment)46. These latter two features are interpreted as suggesting an early neurodevelopmental basis to psychopathy. In children and youth with CU traits, neuropsychological abnormalities (similar to those found in adults with psychopathy) have been found in selective attention, emotional processing, and inhibition, with reduced interference,143 slower reaction times to negative emotional words (versus faster reactions in impulsive children/youth),144 increased reward responsiveness,145 and diminished response inhibition.146 Diminished autonomic reactivity has been found in children and youth with CU traits.147 Abnormal electrodermal response to aversive stimuli at age 3 years was found to be associated with psychopathy in adulthood,148 and impaired electrodermal fear conditioning at age 8 years was associated with aggressive and criminal behavior 20 years later.46 Whereas nonaggressive children show significant increases in fear conditioning from ages 3 to 8 years, aggressive children show a weaker developmental profile, implying diminished maturation of the amygdala. These findings suggest a possible early psychophysiological predisposition to the development of aggressive and antisocial behavior, providing support for a neurodevelopmental contribution to psychopathy.46 Increased testosterone levels have been found in conduct-disordered girls149 and adolescent boys150 without CU traits but not in boys with CU traits.149,151 The relationship between psychopathy and testosterone is further complicated by the fact that testosterone levels change dramatically in puberty and the effects of these changes are largely unknown. Low cortisol levels, and thus lower HPA-axis activity, have been observed in adolescents with CU traits,151 regardless of the presence152 or absence153 of environmental stressors, which may significantly impair social development by reducing responsivity to stressors and decreasing the fear of negative consequences.153

Treatment of Psychopathy/PV

Successful treatment of PV could yield enormous benefits. Psychopathic individuals are estimated to comprise 1% of the population but constitute roughly 15%−25% of the offenders in prison and are responsible for a disproportionate number of brutal crimes. Recent estimates place the national cost of psychopathy at $460 billion a year, roughly 10 times the cost of depression.154 In the United States, the demographics are shifting toward more child and adolescent perpetrators, with increased arrest rates for youths despite decreased rates for adults.45 The younger the perpetrators, the longer their potential active engagement in psychopathic behaviors.45 As the following review of historical and current approaches to treating psychopathy/PV highlights, successful treatment remains an elusive goal. Potentially promising is the possibility that interventions to increase empathy could shift psychopathic individuals and youth with CU traits away from their relational characteristics of mistrust, deception, and manipulation.

Historical and Current Treatment Approaches

Can psychopathy be cured? Historically, treatment efforts have involved significant difficulties, including extremely limited therapeutic rapport owing to a lack of bonding and high deceptiveness in this population56; radically limited motivation for treatment due to a lack of guilt and remorse155; negative treatment outcomes, with worsening of psychopathy because training of socioemotional skills may improve a psychopathic individual’s criminal strategy and capacity to avoid legal detention155; and unreliable and simplistic measurements of treatment outcomes. The limited body of controlled outcome studies is suboptimal. Reasons include the clustering of all patients/inmates considered personality disordered, failure to control for comorbid disorders, lack of control groups, sample sizes too small for statistical significance, and generic treatment modalities.

Early treatment attempts included now-obsolete lobotomies156 and ECT,157 as well as punitive strategies, used in earlier community treatments, which were found to have negative effects, with youth and adults becoming more violent, more manipulative, and more likely to reoffend.158 Pharmacotherapy is found to be efficacious in some individuals with comorbid psychiatric disorders, albeit as a result of improved mood and diminished impulsivity rather than psychopathic traits.159 For example, lithium has been found to reduce irritability in chronically aggressive prisoners, in turn reducing their impulsive aggression, but not their predatory behaviors or overall recidivism.160 Treatment with antidepressants (sertraline) also reduced impulsivity but not fearlessness and dominance of others.161 Treatment with benzodiazepines had negative results,159 presumably because of increased disinhibition and aggression. Although long-term outcomes are mostly unknown, current nonpharmacological treatments include various forms of positive reinforcement, social-skills training, anger management, and cognitive-behavioral and dialectical-behavioral therapy, implemented in prison settings or treatment communities.162,163 Supportive and nurturing approaches are most effective in youth, thus supporting early interventions.164,165 Outcome studies for insight-oriented psychotherapy indicate self-reported improvements that did not correlate with reduced recidivism or changes in psychological traits.166 Although group therapy is commonly used, it lacks reliable outcome studies167 and can even have deleterious effects.168 More recent approaches advocate multimodal interventions, with combined individual, group, and family treatments.164 Notably, typically little information is available about whether any reported improvements transfer to real-life situations. Overall, treatment programs specifically tailored to psychopathy and its affective and interpersonal deficits are scarce.169,170 Efficacy of treatment is typically evaluated based on treatment compliance and recidivism,155 rather than on any positive effects on the affective and interpersonal facets of psychopathy.164,171

Potential Treatments to Increase Empathy

Recent studies have begun to compare treatment of predatory versus impulsive offenders, with group classification based on volumetric measures of gray matter.172,173 Although patients with impulsive personality traits appear to be responsive to existing therapies, traditional treatments for predatory offenders do not appear to be helpful.173 Using volumetric measures, one study found increased empathic responding in psychopathic individuals on a task of intentional effort to empathize.174 Other authors have advocated an overt cognitive process of attention reallocation in an effort to activate top-down triggering of empathic responsiveness. It remains unclear, however, what cognitive, psychological, and emotional processes are involved in intentional effortful empathizing and also whether direct interventions to increase empathy actually stimulate empathy or merely the mimicking of empathic responding. A potentially better approach is to activate empathic processes less directly and explicitly, thereby possibly bypassing the mistrust and deceptiveness typical of psychopathic individuals. Can the brain be trained or induced to become more empathic? In the general population, this question has been addressed from a number of perspectives.

Cognitive reframing/reappraisal therapy, requiring the imagination of positive outcomes to suffering, can increase empathic responsiveness through activation of higher cortical functions, which downregulate amygdala activation, reducing cortisol secretion and autonomic fear activation.175 Stable and empathic attachment in anxious children can develop by priming with words, memories, or stories of secure attachment.176 Deliberate affective regulation, with increased PFC and decreased amygdala activation,177 suggests inhibitory top-down influences of cortical prefrontal projections to the amygdala.178 Eight weeks of mindfulness meditation can induce neuroplastic changes in the anterior cingulate cortex, insula, temporoparietal junction, and frontolimbic network, with associated increases in attention regulation, body awareness, emotional regulation, and self-other perspective.179,180 Although it requires years of sustained practice (but irrespective of meditator’s age), long-term mindfulness training induces volumetric changes in the insula, amygdala, and right temporoparietal junction, with resulting increased empathy.175 The significance of these findings depends on whether they can be meaningfully applied to the population of psychopathic offenders.

Any promising intervention with youth with CU traits or adults who exhibit PV would need to address both possible hypoarousal in the limbic region (associated with reduced fear and stress sensitivity, reduced responsiveness to punishment, and reduced empathic resonance) and possible neurocognitive rigidity, impaired attentional mechanisms, and impaired attachment subserved by the OFC and vmPFC. Possible hormonal and neurotransmitter imbalances further reinforcing such mechanisms must also be considered. To our knowledge, no such comprehensive treatment program has been tested or implemented to date.

Discussion

Review of the neurobiology of empathy has revealed that significant aspects of empathic responsiveness are present at birth and continue to mature throughout childhood and adolescence, in the context of interpersonal relationships. Maturation involves a progressive shift along the dorsolateral PFC-limbic pathway, from more activation of limbic structures early in development to more activation of frontal regions later in development. The extent of this shift is affected by individual predisposition toward autonomic arousal, emotional reactivity, and strength of executive functions.

Although the affective and cognitive components of empathy are dissociable, their interplay allows for emotional regulation. Mature empathic sensitivity depends on the functional integration of these components in the service of relationships and goal-directed social behavior. Social bonding, attachment, and empathy are interconnected at the neurobiological level by the modulatory effects of hormones, with increased oxytocin levels and increased HPA activity correlating positively with more secure attachment and an increased capacity for empathy. Secure attachment and empathic responsiveness alike stimulate the brain reward pathways, which become self-reinforcing.

Whereas IV has been more clearly linked to increased limbic activation with resulting heightened emotional arousal and diminished frontal activation with resulting disinhibition, PV presents a more complex profile. No single region, whether structurally impaired or functionally diminished, will result in psychopathy or in specific cognitive or affective aspects of psychopathy. A recent theoretical paradigm points to limbic hypoarousal, leading to shallow affect and diminished fear, paired with intact or even overactive frontal circuitry, resulting in increased cognitive rigidity, increased hyperattention to selective targets, and difficulties with attention shifting and flexible behavior, possibly leading to obsessive fixations and calculated actions. Further findings, although limited, suggest that psychopathic individuals’ reduced fear, reduced sensitivity to punishment, enhanced sensitivity to reward, and hyporesponsivity to stressors—all of which affect their decision-making behavior—may be linked to the combined effect of reduced cortisol, increased testosterone levels, and reduced HPA activity.

The frontotemporal/limbic/hormonal interplay is likely the main factor in the emergence of PV, but this explanation may still be too simplistic. Whereas impulsive perpetrators typically feel remorse or guilt, predatory perpetrators typically do not. Instead, they tend to feel most engaged and perhaps most “alive” while executing their plans or reliving their experiences through crime-scene revisitations, photos, or other types of “souvenirs” from their crimes and in ways that suggest a radical distortion of the experience of bonding. Such distortions—which, to the best of our knowledge, are poorly understood and scarcely acknowledged—may be what ultimately lead to those behavioral expressions of PV that are described as “evil.”

Such radical deficits of bonding point to a very early etiology of psychopathy and support proposed neurodevelopmental hypotheses.46 Such hypotheses take into account the fact that psychopathic behaviors manifest early in life; continue relatively consistently during childhood, adolescence, and, overall, across time181; and are mostly resistant to conventional treatments.163 Significantly, people who suffer neurological damage at a very early age exhibit characteristics that most closely resemble psychopathy, suggesting that psychopathy is associated with impairments in brain functioning before moral socialization and social bonding.127 In addition, psychosocial, demographic, and head-injury measures alone have not accounted for the structural and functional brain impairments observed in psychopathy.46 A neurodevelopmental hypothesis must also consider the fact that male individuals are much more likely than female individuals to commit certain types of violent crimes.182 Female individuals tend to be more empathic as a result of their evolutionary biological role in the reproduction and care of infants.183 This is true for female children as well, who score higher than male children in empathic concern.184 Although they are still speculative, recent observations suggest neurodevelopmental abnormalities in both juvenile and adult psychopathic offenders.46

Only very recent studies have compared predatory versus impulsive offenders, basing group classification on volumetric measures of gray matter.172,173 These studies show that whereas impulsive offenders can be treated with existing therapies, traditional treatments for predatory offenders may be useless. The interpersonal and affective aspects of psychopathy have only very recently come to the attention of treatment theories and remain to be addressed in actual interventions.

Given that mindfulness training induces neuroplastic changes in the frontolimbic network and increases emotional regulation, attentional skills, and empathic responsiveness, it potentially represents, at least in theory, a more comprehensive and targeted approach to treating the interpersonal/affective empathic deficits in psychopathy. However, because neuroplastic and behavioral changes are a slow process, requiring years of sustained mindfulness practice, this approach is likely suitable only as a very early intervention.

Within this context, the use of prospective longitudinal studies to identify and assess children and adolescents with CU traits has important implications for the prevention and management of adult psychopathy. Increasing the specificity of target theoretical constructs may increase the replicability of findings and produce more converging results. PV and IV distinction, cognitive and affective empathy dissociation, antisocial and psychopathic personality differences should all be taken into consideration in designing future research. Studies are needed that investigate the role of hormones and neurotransmitters and their interplay with neural mechanisms, as well as the precursors, risk factors, and correlates of CU traits in early infancy and in longitudinal designs.

In terms of treatment, we recommend careful assessment of participants in order to better tailor interventions. Best results may be expected when assessments and treatments take place before the second critical period of neuronal plasticity of early adolescence185,186 and differentially target the cognitive and affective aspects of empathic deficits in psychopathy.

Studies are needed that assess the behavioral, psychological, and neurological outcomes of mindfulness training and related types of cognitive-behavioral therapies among incarcerated youth who do or do not exhibit CU traits and/or PV. Such studies should aim to evaluate changes in neural connectivity, cognitive function, cortical thickness, affective reactivity, self-regulation, and social relationships as a result of treatment. This has the potential to uncover an important behavioral intervention, with implications for neural plasticity.

More broadly, we hope that the growing understanding of the neurobiological basis of PV and associated criminal behavior encourages a dialogue on the role of neuroscience in criminal justice, on the implications for punishment versus potential prevention and treatments, and on prediction of violence and risk assessment187 and leads to educational outreach efforts to targeted audiences spanning legal, scientific, and policy fields.

Conclusions

The behavioral expressions of empathy in bonding, attachment, and prosocial behaviors and of deficits in empathy in PV and psychopathic behaviors share significant neural substrates. This, in turn, points to a new way of thinking about their genesis.

Empathic processing is primarily related to the homeostatic functioning of the OFC/vmPFC-limbic pathways and to the reciprocal influence of the HPA axis, oxytocin, and brain reward mechanisms. CU traits, PV, and psychopathic behaviors are tightly linked to these same structures. The functional imbalance of the OFC/vmPFC-limbic pathways leads to the cognitive and interpersonal/affective aspects of psychopathy. Diminished HPA activation and reduced cortisol levels result in increased stress tolerance and diminished fear. This may also potentially affect the ongoing capacity to form attachments and activate the brain reward system during interpersonal interactions. Thus, the inverse relationship between empathy and PV shares similar neuroanatomical substrates. A corollary of this is that strengthening empathy, which enhances bonding, might result in diminished PV.

Individuals with psychopathy are typically viewed as resistant to treatment. Significant conceptual advances have occurred in recent years, particularly regarding the relationship between PV and empathy. Our review suggests that more targeted interventions aimed at specific features of psychopathy might lead to better outcomes, with maximal effectiveness in the context of very early interventions. Continued efforts to identify, assess, and treat children and adolescents with CU traits have important implications for preventing and managing adult psychopathy. Although the specific aspects of deficiencies in empathic processing in psychopathy remain poorly understood, it is clear that an important relationship exists between empathy and PV, which may be essential in the development of treatments for this disorder.

From the Depts. of Psychiatry (DC, JE) and Neurology (BHP), Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA; the Center for Law, Brain, and Behavior (DC, JE, BHP) and the Law & Psychiatry Service (JE), Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston; and the Dept. of Neurology, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA (BHP).
Send correspondence to Dr. Chialant; e-mail:

This work was partially supported by the Massachusetts General Hospital Center for Law, Brain, and Behavior and the Sidney R. Baer Jr. Foundation.

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

References

1 Baskin JH, Edersheim JG, Price BH: Is a picture worth a thousand words? Neuroimaging in the courtroom. Am J Law Med 2007; 33:239–269Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Singer T, Lamm C: The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009; 1156:81–96Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3 de Vignemont F, Singer T: The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10:435–441Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Zaki J, Ochsner KN, Ochsner K: The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15:675–680Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

5 Leppänen JM, Nelson CA: Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10:37–47Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Harrison NA, Singer T, Rotshtein P, et al.: Pupillary contagion: central mechanisms engaged in sadness processing. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2006; 1:5–17Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7 Geangu E, Benga O, Stahl D, et al.: Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behav Dev 2010; 33:279–288Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Decety J: The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Dev Neurosci 2010; 32:257–267Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9 Reniers RL, Corcoran R, Drake R, et al.: The QCAE: a Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy. J Pers Assess 2011; 93:84–95Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Price BH, Daffner KR, Stowe RM, et al.: The comportmental learning disabilities of early frontal lobe damage. Brain 1990; 113:1383–1393Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11 Bickart KC, Brickhouse M, Negreira A, et al.: Atrophy in distinct corticolimbic networks in frontotemporal dementia relates to social impairments measured using the Social Impairment Rating Scale. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2014; 85:438–448Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12 Sturm VE, Rosen HJ, Allison S, et al.: Self-conscious emotion deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Brain 2006; 129:2508–2516Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Shamay-Tsoory SG: The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 2011; 17:18–24Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

14 Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, et al.: Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011; 35:903–911Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15 Eres R, Decety J, Louis WR, et al.: Individual differences in local gray matter density are associated with differences in affective and cognitive empathy. Neuroimage 2015; 117:305–310Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16 Eisenberg N, Eggum ND: Empathic responding: sympathy and personal distress, in The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Edited by Decety J, Ickes W. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2009, pp 71–83CrossrefGoogle Scholar

17 Gonzalez-Liencres C, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Brüne M: Towards a neuroscience of empathy: ontogeny, phylogeny, brain mechanisms, context and psychopathology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013; 37:1537–1548Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18 Rizzolatti G, Sinigaglia C: The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11:264–274Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19 Keysers C, Kaas JH, Gazzola V: Somatosensation in social perception. Nat Rev Neurosci 2010; 11:417–428Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

20 Lamm C, Singer T: The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Struct Funct 2010; 214:579–591Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21 Buckner RL, Carroll DC: Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2007; 11:49–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22 Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim AS: The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 2009; 21:489–510Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23 Fan Y, Han S: Temporal dynamic of neural mechanisms involved in empathy for pain: an event-related brain potential study. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46:160–173Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24 Nichols SR, Svetlova M, Brownell CA: The role of social understanding and empathic disposition in young children’s responsiveness to distress in parents and peers. Cogn Brain Behav 2009; 13:449–478MedlineGoogle Scholar

25 Saxe RR, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Scholz J, et al.: Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Dev 2009; 80:1197–1209Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26 Killgore WD, Yurgelun-Todd DA: Unconscious processing of facial affect in children and adolescents. Soc Neurosci 2007; 2:28–47Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27 Decety J, Michalska KJ: Neurodevelopmental changes in the circuits underlying empathy and sympathy from childhood to adulthood. Dev Sci 2010; 13:886–899Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28 Hurliman E, Nagode JC, Pardo JV: Double dissociation of exteroceptive and interoceptive feedback systems in the orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex of humans. J Neurosci 2005; 25:4641–4648Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29 Swick D, Ashley V, Turken AU: Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neurosci 2008; 9:102Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

30 Tabibnia G, Lieberman MD, Craske MG: The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images. Emotion 2008; 8:307–317Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31 Ochsner KN, Gross JJ: The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9:242–249Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32 Plutchik R: Evolutionary bases of empathy, in Empathy and Its Development. Edited by Eisenberg N, Strayer J. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1987, pp 37–46Google Scholar

33 Weinfield NS, Srouffe LA, Egeland B, et al.: Individual differences in infant-caregiver attachment, in Handbook of Attachment. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York, Guilford Press, 2010, pp 78–101Google Scholar

34 Burnette JL, Davis DE, Green JD, et al.: Insecure attachment and depressive symptoms: the mediating role of rumination, empathy, and forgiveness. Pers Individ Dif 2009; 46:276–280CrossrefGoogle Scholar

35 Numan M, Sheehan TP: Neuroanatomical circuitry for mammalian maternal behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1997; 807:101–125Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36 Gunnar M, Quevedo K: The neurobiology of stress and development. Annu Rev Psychol 2007; 58:145–173Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37 Gunnar MR, Donzella B: Social regulation of the cortisol levels in early human development. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2002; 27:199–220Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38 Seifritz E, Esposito F, Neuhoff JG, et al.: Differential sex-independent amygdala response to infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54:1367–1375Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39 Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, et al.: Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54:1389–1398Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

40 Heinrichs M, von Dawans B, Domes G: Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol 2009; 30:548–557Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

41 Broad KD, Curley JP, Keverne EB: Mother-infant bonding and the evolution of mammalian social relationships. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2006; 361:2199–2214Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

42 Strathearn L, Fonagy P, Amico J, et al.: Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009; 34:2655–2666Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

43 Moll J, Krueger F, Zahn R, et al.: Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103:15623–15628Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

44 Maestripieri D: Neurobiology of social behavior, in Primate Neuroethology. Edited by Platt MI, Ghazanfar A. New York, Oxford University Press, 2010, pp 359–384CrossrefGoogle Scholar

45 Filley CM, Price BH, Nell V, et al.: Toward an understanding of violence: neurobehavioral aspects of unwarranted physical aggression: Aspen Neurobehavioral Conference consensus statement. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 2001; 14:1–14MedlineGoogle Scholar

46 Gao Y, Glenn AL, Schug RA, et al.: The neurobiology of psychopathy: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Can J Psychiatry 2009; 54:813–823Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

47 Siever LJ: Neurobiology of aggression and violence. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:429–442Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

48 Blair RJ: Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71:727–731Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

49 Declercq F, Willemsen J, Audenaert K, et al.: Psychopathy and predatory violence in homicide, violent, and sexual offences: factor and facet relations. Leg Criminol Psychol 2012; 17:59–74Google Scholar

50 Walsh Z, Swogger MT, Walsh T, et al.: Psychopathy and violence: increasing specificity. Neth J Psychol 2007; 63:125MedlineGoogle Scholar

51 Hare RD: Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Toronto, Multi-Health Systems, 2003Google Scholar

52 Schouten R, Silver J: Almost a Psychopath: Do I (or Does Someone I Know) Have a Problem With Manipulation and Lack of Empathy? Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press, 2012Google Scholar

53 Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D: Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain 2009; 132:617–627Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

54 van Langen MAM, Wissink IB, van Vugt ES, et al.: The relation between empathy and offending: a meta-analysis. Aggress Violent Behav 2014; 19:179–189CrossrefGoogle Scholar

55 Glenn AL, Johnson AK, Raine A: Antisocial personality disorder: a current review. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2013; 15:427Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

56 Cleckley HM: The mask of sanity. Postgrad Med 1951; 9:193–197Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

57 Damasio AR: Descartes’ error and the future of human life. Sci Am 1994; 271:144Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

58 Koenigs M, Young L, Adolphs R, et al.: Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements. Nature 2007; 446:908–911Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

59 Anderson SW, Bechara A, Damasio H, et al.: Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:1032–1037Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

60 Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H: Individuals with sociopathic behavior caused by frontal damage fail to respond autonomically to social stimuli. Behav Brain Res 1990; 41:81–94Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

61 Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, et al.: Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5473–5481Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

62 Angrilli A, Mauri A, Palomba D, et al.: Startle reflex and emotion modulation impairment after a right amygdala lesion. Brain 1996; 119:1991–2000Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

63 Adolphs R: Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2002; 12:169–177Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

64 Damasio A: Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York, Avon Books, 1994Google Scholar

65 Bernhardt BC, Singer T: The neural basis of empathy. Annu Rev Neurosci 2012; 35:1–23Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

66 Yoder KJ, Porges EC, Decety J: Amygdala subnuclei connectivity in response to violence reveals unique influences of individual differences in psychopathic traits in a nonforensic sample. Hum Brain Mapp 2015; 36:1417–1428Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

67 Appelbaum PS, Scurich N: Impact of behavioral genetic evidence on the adjudication of criminal behavior. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 2014; 42:91–100MedlineGoogle Scholar

68 Raine A: Genetics and crime, in The Psychopathology of Crime: Criminal Behavior as a Clinical Disorder. London, Academic Press, 1993, pp 47–49CrossrefGoogle Scholar

69 Sabol SZ, Hu S, Hamer D: A functional polymorphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Hum Genet 1998; 103:273–279Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

70 Caspi A, McClay J, Moffitt TE, et al.: Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 2002; 297:851–854Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

71 Ferguson CJ, Beaver KM: Natural-born killers: the genetic origins of extreme violence. Aggress Violent Behav 2009; 14:286–294CrossrefGoogle Scholar

72 Plomin R: The role of inheritance in behavior. Science 1990; 248:183–188Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

73 Frazzetto G, Di Lorenzo G, Carola V, et al.: Early trauma and increased risk for physical aggression during adulthood: the moderating role of MAOA genotype. PLoS One 2007; 2:e486Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

74 Fergusson DM, Boden JM, Horwood LJ, et al.: Moderating role of the MAOA genotype in antisocial behaviour. Br J Psychiatry 2012; 200:116–123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

75 Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW: On the use and misuse of genomic and neuroimaging science in forensic psychiatry: current roles and future directions. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2011; 20:533–546Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

76 Edersheim JG, Price BH, Smoller JW: ‘Your Honor, my genes made me do it.’ Wall Street Journal, Oct. 21, 2012Google Scholar

77 Hare RD: Without Conscience: The Disturbing World of the Psychopaths Among Us. New York, Simon & Schuster, 1993Google Scholar

78 Bassarath L: Neuroimaging studies of antisocial behaviour. Can J Psychiatry 2001; 46:728–732Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

79 Bufkin JL, Luttrell VR: Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice. Trauma Violence Abuse 2005; 6:176–191Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

80 Contreras-Rodríguez O, Pujol J, Batalla I, et al.: Functional connectivity bias in the prefrontal cortex of psychopaths. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 78:647–655Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

81 Koenigs M: The role of prefrontal cortex in psychopathy. Rev Neurosci 2012; 23:253–262Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

82 Raine A, Meloy JR, Bihrle S, et al.: Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers. Behav Sci Law 1998; 16:319–332Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

83 Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Hare RD, et al.: Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50:677–684Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

84 Raine A, Lencz T, Taylor K, et al.: Corpus callosum abnormalities in psychopathic antisocial individuals. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:1134–1142Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

85 Raine A, Lencz T, Bihrle S, et al.: Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:119–127, discussion 128–129Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

86 Smith A: Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. Psychol Rec 2006; 56:3–21CrossrefGoogle Scholar

87 Decety J, Michalska KJ, Akitsuki Y, et al.: Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Biol Psychol 2009; 80:203–211Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

88 Shamay-Tsoory SG, Harari H, Aharon-Peretz J, et al.: The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex 2010; 46:668–677Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

89 de Oliveira-Souza R, Hare RD, Bramati IE, et al.: Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain: fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage 2008; 40:1202–1213Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

90 Müller JL, Gänssbauer S, Sommer M, et al.: Gray matter changes in right superior temporal gyrus in criminal psychopaths. Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. Psychiatry Res 2008; 163:213–222Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

91 Ermer E, Cope LM, Nyalakanti PK, et al.: Aberrant paralimbic gray matter in incarcerated male adolescents with psychopathic traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 52:94–103.e3Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

92 Pardini DA, Raine A, Erickson K, et al.: Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75:73–80Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

93 Boccardi M, Bocchetta M, Aronen HJ, et al.: Atypical nucleus accumbens morphology in psychopathy: another limbic piece in the puzzle. Int J Law Psychiatry 2013; 36:157–167Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

94 Raine A, Ishikawa SS, Arce E, et al.: Hippocampal structural asymmetry in unsuccessful psychopaths. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:185–191Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

95 Yang Y, Raine A, Lencz T, et al.: Volume reduction in prefrontal gray matter in unsuccessful criminal psychopaths. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:1103–1108Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

96 Archer J: The influence of testosterone on human aggression. Br J Psychol 1991; 82:1–28Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

97 Dabbs JM Jr, Frady RL, Carr TS, et al.: Saliva testosterone and criminal violence in young adult prison inmates. Psychosom Med 1987; 49:174–182Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

98 van Honk J, Schutter DJ: Unmasking feigned sanity: a neurobiological model of emotion processing in primary psychopathy. Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2006; 11:285–306Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

99 Rosell DR, Siever LJ: The neurobiology of aggression and violence. CNS Spectr 2015; 20:254–279Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

100 Shirtcliff EA, Vitacco MJ, Graf AR, et al.: Neurobiology of empathy and callousness: implications for the development of antisocial behavior. Behav Sci Law 2009; 27:137–171Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

101 Cima M, Smeets T, Jelicic M: Self-reported trauma, cortisol levels, and aggression in psychopathic and non-psychopathic prison inmates. Biol Psychol 2008; 78:75–86Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

102 Holi M, Auvinen-Lintunen L, Lindberg N, et al.: Inverse correlation between severity of psychopathic traits and serum cortisol levels in young adult violent male offenders. Psychopathology 2006; 39:102–104Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

103 van Honk J, Schutter DJ, Hermans EJ, et al.: Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and reward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004; 29:937–943Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

104 Volavka J: The neurobiology of violence: an update. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999; 11:307–314LinkGoogle Scholar

105 Vitale JE, Brinkley CA, Hiatt KD, et al.: Abnormal selective attention in psychopathic female offenders. Neuropsychology 2007; 21:301–312Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

106 Kosson DS, Miller SK, Byrnes KA, et al.: Testing neuropsychological hypotheses for cognitive deficits in psychopathic criminals: a study of global-local processing. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13:267–276Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

107 Blair KS, Richell RA, Mitchell DG, et al.: They know the words, but not the music: affective and semantic priming in individuals with psychopathy. Biol Psychol 2006; 73:114–123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

108 Koolhof R, Loeber R, Wei EH, et al.: Inhibition deficits of serious delinquent boys of low intelligence. Crim Behav Ment Health 2007; 17:274–292Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

109 van Honk J, Hermans EJ, Putman P, et al.: Defective somatic markers in sub-clinical psychopathy. Neuroreport 2002; 13:1025–1027Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

110 Schmitt WA, Brinkley CA, Newman JP: Testing Damasio’s somatic marker hypothesis with psychopathic individuals: risk takers or risk averse? J Abnorm Psychol 1999; 108:538–543Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

111 Lösel F, Schmucker M: Psychopathy, risk taking, and attention: a differentiated test of the somatic marker hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol 2004; 113:522–529Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

112 Williamson S, Harpur TJ, Hare RD: Abnormal processing of affective words by psychopaths. Psychophysiology 1991; 28:260–273Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

113 Hervé HF, Hayes PJ, Hare RD: Psychopathy and sensitivity to the emotional polarity of metaphorical statements. Pers Individ Dif 2003; 35:1497–1507CrossrefGoogle Scholar

114 Ishikawa SS, Raine A, Lencz T, et al.: Autonomic stress reactivity and executive functions in successful and unsuccessful criminal psychopaths from the community. J Abnorm Psychol 2001; 110:423–432Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

115 Lorber MF: Psychophysiology of aggression, psychopathy, and conduct problems: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2004; 130:531–552Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

116 Pastor MC, Moltó J, Vila J, et al.: Startle reflex modulation, affective ratings and autonomic reactivity in incarcerated Spanish psychopaths. Psychophysiology 2003; 40:934–938Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

117 Kiehl KA, Bates AT, Laurens KR, et al.: Brain potentials implicate temporal lobe abnormalities in criminal psychopaths. J Abnorm Psychol 2006; 115:443–453Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

118 Munro GE, Dywan J, Harris GT, et al.: Response inhibition in psychopathy: the frontal N2 and P3. Neurosci Lett 2007; 418:149–153Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

119 Kiehl KA, Smith AM, Hare RD, et al.: An event-related potential investigation of response inhibition in schizophrenia and psychopathy. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:210–221Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

120 Campanella S, Vanhoolandt ME, Philippot P: Emotional deficit in subjects with psychopathic tendencies as assessed by the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2: an event-related potentials study. Neurosci Lett 2005; 373:26–31Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

121 Dikman ZV, Allen JJ: Error monitoring during reward and avoidance learning in high- and low-socialized individuals. Psychophysiology 2000; 37:43–54Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

122 Munro GE, Dywan J, Harris GT, et al.: ERN varies with degree of psychopathy in an emotion discrimination task. Biol Psychol 2007; 76:31–42Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

123 Brazil IA, de Bruijn ER, Bulten BH, et al.: Early and late components of error monitoring in violent offenders with psychopathy. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 65:137–143Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

124 Patrick CJ: Getting to the heart of psychopathy, in The Psychopath: Theory, Research, and Practice. Edited by Herve HYJ. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2007, pp 207–252Google Scholar

125 Lykken DT: The Antisocial Personalities. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1995Google Scholar

126 Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG: Psychopathy, startle blink modulation, and electrodermal reactivity in twin men. Psychophysiology 2005; 42:753–762Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

127 Brower MC, Price BH: Neuropsychiatry of frontal lobe dysfunction in violent and criminal behaviour: a critical review. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71:720–726Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

128 Frick PJ: Extending the construct of psychopathy to youth: implications for understanding, diagnosing, and treating antisocial children and adolescents. Can J Psychiatry 2009; 54:803–812Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

129 Crapanzano AM, Frick PJ, Terranova AM: Patterns of physical and relational aggression in a school-based sample of boys and girls. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2010; 38:433–445Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

130 Frick PJ, Dickens C: Current perspectives on conduct disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2006; 8:59–72Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

131 Pardini D, Lochman J, Wells K: Negative emotions and alcohol use initiation in high-risk boys: the moderating effect of good inhibitory control. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2004; 32:505–518Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

132 Viding E, Larsson H, Jones AP: Quantitative genetic studies of antisocial behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2008; 363:2519–2527Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

133 Frick PJ, White SF: Research review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49:359–375Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

134 Mathias CW, Stanford MS, Marsh DM, et al.: Characterizing aggressive behavior with the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale among adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res 2007; 151:231–242Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

135 Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DG, et al.: Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:712–720Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

136 Blair RJ: The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat Rev Neurosci 2013; 14:786–799Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

137 Marsh AA, Finger EC, Fowler KA, et al.: Empathic responsiveness in amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex in youths with psychopathic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013; 54:900–910Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

138 Marsh AA, Finger EC, Fowler KA, et al.: Reduced amygdala-orbitofrontal connectivity during moral judgments in youths with disruptive behavior disorders and psychopathic traits. Psychiatry Res 2011; 194:279–286Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

139 Finger EC, Marsh AA, Mitchell DG, et al.: Abnormal ventromedial prefrontal cortex function in children with psychopathic traits during reversal learning. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:586–594Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

140 Kruesi MJ, Casanova MF, Mannheim G, et al.: Reduced temporal lobe volume in early onset conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res 2004; 132:1–11Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

141 Yang Y, Narr KL, Baker LA, et al.: Frontal and striatal alterations associated with psychopathic traits in adolescents. Psychiatry Res 2015; 231:333–340Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

142 Yang Y, Wang P, Baker LA, et al.: Thicker temporal cortex associates with a developmental trajectory for psychopathic traits in adolescents. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0127025Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

143 Vitale JE, Newman JP, Bates JE, et al.: Deficient behavioral inhibition and anomalous selective attention in a community sample of adolescents with psychopathic traits and low-anxiety traits. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2005; 33:461–470Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

144 Loney BR, Frick PJ, Clements CB, et al.: Callous-unemotional traits, impulsivity, and emotional processing in adolescents with antisocial behavior problems. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2003; 32:66–80Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

145 Scerbo A, Raine A, O’Brien M, et al.: Reward dominance and passive avoidance learning in adolescent psychopaths. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1990; 18:451–463Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

146 Roussy ST, Toupin J: Behavioral inhibition deficits in juvenile psychopaths. Aggress Behav 2000; 26:413–424CrossrefGoogle Scholar

147 Anastassiou-Hadjicharalambous X, Warden D: Physiologically-indexed and self-perceived affective empathy in conduct-disordered children high and low on callous-unemotional traits. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2008; 39:503–517Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

148 Glenn AL, Raine A, Venables PH, et al.: Early temperamental and psychophysiological precursors of adult psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol 2007; 116:508–518Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

149 Pajer K, Tabbah R, Gardner W, et al.: Adrenal androgen and gonadal hormone levels in adolescent girls with conduct disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2006; 31:1245–1256Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

150 Maras A, Laucht M, Gerdes D, et al.: Association of testosterone and dihydrotestosterone with externalizing behavior in adolescent boys and girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2003; 28:932–940Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

151 Loney BR, Butler MA, Lima EN, et al.: The relation between salivary cortisol, callous-unemotional traits, and conduct problems in an adolescent non-referred sample. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:30–36Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

152 van Goozen SH, Matthys W, Cohen-Kettenis PT, et al.: Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and autonomic nervous system activity in disruptive children and matched controls. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:1438–1445Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

153 Hawes DJ, Brennan J, Dadds MR: Cortisol, callous-unemotional traits, and pathways to antisocial behavior. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2009; 22:357–362Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

154 Kahn J: Can you call a 9-year-old a psychopath? New York Times, May 11, 2012Google Scholar

155 Harris GT, Rice ME: Treatment of psychopathy: a review of empirical findings, in Handbook of Psychopathy. Edited by Patrick C. New York, Guilford Press, 2006, pp 555–572Google Scholar

156 Robin AA: A controlled study of the effects of leucotomy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1958; 21:262–269Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

157 Green E, Silverman D, Geil G: Petit mal electro shock therapy of criminal psychopaths. J Crim Psychopathol 1944; 5:667–695Google Scholar

158 O’Neill ML, Lidz V, Heilbrun K: Adolescents with psychopathic characteristics in a substance abusing cohort: treatment process and outcomes. Law Hum Behav 2003; 27:299–313Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

159 Gardner DL, Cowdry RW: Alprazolam-induced dyscontrol in borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1985; 142:98–100Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

160 Tupin JP, Smith DB, Clanon TL, et al.: The long-term use of lithium in aggressive prisoners. Compr Psychiatry 1973; 14:311–317Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

161 Dunlop BW, DeFife JA, Marx L, et al.: The effects of sertraline on psychopathic traits. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 2011; 26:329–337Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

162 Tew J, Dixon L, Harkins L, et al.: Investigating changes in anger and aggression in offenders with high levels of psychopathic traits attending the Chromis violence reduction programme. Crim Behav Ment Health 2012; 22:191–201Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

163 Felthous AR: The appropriateness of treating psychopathic disorders. CNS Spectr 2015; 20:182–189Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

164 Salekin RT, Tippey JG, Allen AD: Treatment of conduct problem youth with interpersonal callous traits using mental models: measurement of risk and change. Behav Sci Law 2012; 30:470–486Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

165 Skeem JL, Polaschek DL, Patrick CJ, et al.: Psychopathic personality: bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy. Psychol Sci Public Interest 2011; 12:95–162Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

166 Snowden P: Facilities and treatment, in Seminars in Practical Forensic Psychiatry. Edited by Chiswick D, Cope R. London, Gaskell, 1995Google Scholar

167 Jew CC, Clanon TL, Mattocks AL: The effectiveness of group psychotherapy in a correctional institution. Am J Psychiatry 1972; 129:602–605Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

168 Lloyd CD, Hanby LJ, Serin RC: Rehabilitation group coparticipants’ risk levels are associated with offenders’ treatment performance, treatment change, and recidivism. J Consult Clin Psychol 2014; 82:298–311Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

169 Salekin R: Treatment of child and adolescent psychopathy: focusing on change, in Handbook of Child and Adolescent Psychopathy. Edited by Salekin RT, Lynam DR. New York, Guilford Press, 2010, pp 343–373Google Scholar

170 Cummings MA: The neurobiology of psychopathy: recent developments and new directions in research and treatment. CNS Spectr 2015; 20:200–206Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

171 Ribeiro da Silva D, Rijo D, Salekin RT: Child and adolescent psychopathy: assessment issues and treatment needs. Aggress Violent Behav 2013; 18:71–78CrossrefGoogle Scholar

172 De Brito SA, Viding E, Kumari V, et al.: Cool and hot executive function impairments in violent offenders with antisocial personality disorder with and without psychopathy. PLoS One 2013; 8:e65566Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

173 Hsu C: “Cold-hearted” psychopaths are born with distinct brains, existing treatments may be useless. Medical Daily, May 8, 2012Google Scholar

174 Meffert H, Gazzola V, den Boer JA, et al.: Reduced spontaneous but relatively normal deliberate vicarious representations in psychopathy. Brain 2013; 136:2550–2562Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

175 Davidson RJ: The neurobiology of compassion, in Wisdom and Compassion in Psychotherapy: Deepening Mindfulness in Clinical Practice, 1st ed. Edited by Germer C, Siegel RD. New York, Guilford Publications, 2012, p 111Google Scholar

176 Siegel DJ: The Mindful Brain: Reflection and Attunement in the Cultivation of Well-Being, Norton Series on Interpersonal Neurobiology. New York, W.W. Norton, 2007Google Scholar

177 Harenski CL, Hamann S: Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions related to moral violations. Neuroimage 2006; 30:313–324Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

178 Banks SJ, Eddy KT, Angstadt M, et al.: Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2007; 2:303–312Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

179 Lutz J, Herwig U, Opialla S, et al.: Mindfulness and emotion regulation--an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2014; 9:776–785Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

180 Fox KC, Nijeboer S, Dixon ML, et al.: Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2014; 43:48–73Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

181 Moffitt TE: Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy. Psychol Rev 1993; 100:674–701Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

182 Compton WM, Conway KP, Stinson FS, et al.: Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity of DSM-IV antisocial personality syndromes and alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. J Clin Psychiatry 2005; 66:677–685Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

183 Montagne B, Kessels RP, Frigerio E, et al.: Sex differences in the perception of affective facial expressions: do men really lack emotional sensitivity? Cogn Process 2005; 6:136–141Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

184 Knafo A, Zahn-Waxler C, Van Hulle C, et al.: The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion 2008; 8:737–752Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

185 Cicchetti D: Neural plasticity, sensitive periods, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol 2015; 27:319–320Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

186 Herpers PC, Scheepers FE, Bons DM, et al.: The cognitive and neural correlates of psychopathy and especially callous-unemotional traits in youths: a systematic review of the evidence. Dev Psychopathol 2014; 26:245–273CrossrefGoogle Scholar

187 Glenn AL, Raine A: Neurocriminology: implications for the punishment, prediction and prevention of criminal behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 2014; 15:54–63Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar