The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Regular ArticleFull Access

A Two-Year Follow-Up Study of Remote Memory in Alzheimer's Disease

Published Online:

Abstract

The authors assessed long-term changes in autobiographical and public remote memory in a series of 21 patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and 10 age-comparable healthy comparison subjects who underwent two evaluations, with an interval ranging from 24 to 36 months. The assessment consisted of the Remote and Autobiographical Memory Scales and the Buschke Selective Reminding Test. The AD group showed a significantly greater decline on both types of remote memory than the comparison group. Alzheimer’s disease patients performed significantly better on recognition than on free-recall, suggesting more severe retrieval than encoding/storage deficits. The decline in anterograde verbal memory correlated significantly with the decline in autobiographical memory, suggesting a common final pathway for both deficits.

During the past decade, various studies have demonstrated significant deficits in autobiographical and public remote memory in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD).1,2 We reported more severe deficits in AD patients than in age-comparable healthy subjects on both free recall and recognition memory for public events and famous people. These deficits were of similar magnitude across the four decades assessed: 1950s, 1960s, 1970s, and 1980s.2 AD patients also showed significantly more severe deficits than age-comparable healthy subjects on both the free recall and recognition modalities on the Autobiographical Memory Scale. Additionally, there was a significant gradient, with AD patients showing better recall of early life events (childhood and adolescence) than late life events.2 On both public remote and autobiographical memory, AD patients showed better performance on recognition than free-recall, suggesting that both retrieval deficits and damage to memory traces may be involved in the mechanism of remote memory impairment in AD.2

Greene and Hodges1 were the first to conduct a longitudinal (1-year) follow-up study of remote memory in AD. They found that only public memory had a significant decline during the follow-up period, although public and autobiographical memory were impaired in AD. Greene and Hodges assessed autobiographical memory using the Autobiographic Memory Interview, which is based on asking patients to provide personal semantic and incidental data. An important limitation of this approach is that memories produced by patients are not usually verified for accuracy. Lack of autobiographical memory decline in the Greene and Hodge1 study could have also resulted from the relatively short follow-up period.

To examine long-term changes in autobiographical and public remote memory in dementia, we assessed a series of 21 AD patients and 10 age-comparable healthy subjects twice, with an interval ranging from 24 to 36 months. Both groups were assessed using the Autobiographical Memory Scale, which consists of specific questions from personal history verified by first-degree relatives for accurate responses; the Remote Memory Scale, which assesses memories for famous people and well-known events from four decades (1950s to 1980s);2 and the Buschke Selective Reminding Test, an examination of verbal anterograde memory.

PATIENTS AND METHODS

Patients

Our initial study included 44 patients meeting the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke (NINCDS)-Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association (ADRDA)3 criteria for probable AD, and 24 age-comparable healthy individuals. Patients with a history of stroke, one or more focal lesions on a T1-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scan or a Hachinski Ischemic score >44 were excluded. Twenty-one patients had completed both the Remote and the Autobiographical Memory Scales; 19 patients had completed the Remote Memory Scale only; and four patients had completed the Autobiographical Memory Scale only. Two years after the initial assessment, all patients and healthy comparison subjects with the baseline evaluation were contacted for a follow-up assessment. Seventeen of the 21 AD patients with a baseline full remote memory assessment, four patients with a baseline Remote Memory Scale assessment only, and 10 of the 24 healthy subjects with an initial evaluation were reassessed between 24 and 36 months following the initial assessment. There were no significant differences between AD patients with or without a follow up on age (mean=74.5 years [SD=6.2] versus 69.6 years [SD=8.1], respectively), years of education (mean=9.8 years [SD=3.2] versus 11.6 [SD=5.0], respectively), and baseline Mini Mental State Exam scores (mean=23.0 [SD=5.0] versus 21.8 [SD=6.5], respectively). Eight of the 14 comparison subjects without a follow up could not be located, and six refused repeated participation (mostly due to time constraints).

Memory Assessment

After informed, written consent was obtained from patients, caregivers, and healthy subjects, memory assessments were conducted by a neuropsychologist blind to group membership using the Remote Memory Scale,2 the Autobiographical Memory Scale,2 and the Buschke Selective Reminding Test.5

Remote Memory Scale.

The Remote Memory Scale consists of two sections: free recall and recognition. Each section contains 40 questions (10 questions per decade: five famous names and five well-known events) for the 1950s, 1960s, 1970s, and 1980s.

Autobiographical Memory Scale.

The Autobiographical Memory Scale consists of 39 questions assessing childhood (N=6), adolescence (N=6), youth (N=9), adulthood (N=9), and recent events (N=9). The test consists of a free recall section, in which patients provide free answers and a recognition section, which includes the above 39 questions and three different answers: the one correct answer (provided by family members) and two semantic foils. The percentage of questions correctly answered served as the metric. We demonstrated the validity and reliability of these scales in AD.

Buschke Selective Reminding Test.

The Buschke Selective Reminding Test measures verbal learning and memory during a multiple trial list learning task. The total recall was used as the outcome measure.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using means and standard deviations (SDs), analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures, post hoc t tests, and Spearman Rank Order correlations. Frequency distributions were calculated with chi-square tests and a Yates’s correction for expected cell sizes less than 5. All p values were two-tailed.

RESULTS

Background Findings

There were no significant between-group differences in age (AD patients: mean=74.5 [SD=6.2], comparison group: mean=72.1 [SD=9.8]), gender (percentage of women: 57% and 50%, respectively), and years of education (mean=11.0 [SD=4.2] and 11.7 [SD=5.1], respectively).

Autobiographical Memory Findings (Table 1)

A 4-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures for the Autobiographical Memory Scale (group [AD versus comparison] × time (initial versus follow-up evaluations) × task (free recall versus recognition) × life periods (childhood, adolescence, youth, adulthood, recent events) showed the following significant main effects and interactions: 1) Group effect (F=8.18, df=1,25, p<0.01); the AD group had a worse performance on the Autobiographical Memory Scale (AMS) than the healthy comparison group, as expected. 2) Task effect (F=10.9, df=1,25, p<0.01); the free recall section was significantly more difficult than the recognition section, as expected. 3) Group × time interaction (F=4.62, df=1,25, p<0.05); the AD group showed a significantly greater decline during follow up than the comparison group. 4) Group × period interaction (F=4.37, df=4,100, p<0.01); AD patients had a significantly better recall for childhood and adolescence than for later periods (Figure 1).

Public Memory Findings (Table 2)

A four-way ANOVA with repeated measures for the Remote Memory Scale (group [AD versus comparison], time [initial versus follow-up evaluations], task (free recall versus recognition); and decades [1950s–1980s] showed the following significant main effects and interactions: 1) The AD group had a significantly worse performance than the comparison group, as expected (Group effect [F=13.9, df=1,29, p<0.001]). 2) The free recall section was significantly more difficult than the recognition section, as expected (Task effect [F=87.8, df=1,29, p<0.0001]). 3) The AD group showed a significantly greater decline during the follow up than the comparison group. (Group × time interaction [F=5.72, df=1,29, p<0.01]).

To examine the presence of a significant gradient for remote memory deficits in AD for famous people and well known events, we calculated a three-way ANOVA with repeated measures. To avoid floor effects, we excluded AD patients (N=7) with a score of 0 on all four decades at the initial evaluation. We found the expected significant time (F = 25.8, df = 1,22, p<0.0001) and task effects (F = 73.1, df = 1,22, p<0.0001) and a significant time × decade interaction (F = 3.83, df = 3,66 p<0.05), which was the result of a significantly better recall for early than late decades at follow up but not at the initial evaluation (Figure 2).

Finally, we calculated the rate of anterograde autobiographical and public remote memory decline in AD using follow-up scores as numerator and baseline scores as denominator. There was a significant correlation between the magnitude of decline in anterograde verbal memory (as assessed with the Buschke Selective Reminding test) and autobiographical memory (Spearman R= 0.63, N=3.28, p<0.01) (Figure 3), but there were no significant correlations between the magnitude of decline in autobiographical versus public remote memory (R=0.03), between anterograde verbal memory and public remote memory (R=0.21), and between the magnitude of decline in Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE) scores and autobiographical memory (R= 0.39).

DISCUSSION

There are several important findings in this study. First, the AD group showed a significantly greater decline than the comparison group on both types of remote memory during a 30-month period. Second, AD patients showed a significantly better performance on the recognition section of both remote memory tasks than on the free recall sections, suggesting a relatively more severe retrieval when compared to storage deficits. On the other hand, there was a parallel decline on free recall and recognition, suggesting that both retrieval and storage of remote memory data have a similar rate of decline in AD. Fourth, there was a significant recall gradient on the public remote memory test (i.e., significantly better recall of earlier than later decades) at follow up but not at the baseline evaluation, suggesting that the presence of a temporal gradient is significantly related to the stage of illness. Finally, there was a significant correlation between the rate of decline for anterograde verbal and autobiographical memory, but there was no significant correlation between the rate of decline in autobiographical and public remote memory.

Several limitations to the study should be noted. First, our relatively high drop-out rate may have biased our results toward less severe cognitive decline in the AD group. Second, we did not measure the rate of decline in anterograde visuospatial memory or the severity of hippocampal atrophy, and we could not determine whether a compensatory contribution of the right hippocampus may influence correlations between autobiographical and verbal anterograde memory.

In their 1-year follow-up study of 24 AD patients, Greene and Hodges reported a significant decline in famous face and name identification but not in autobiographical memory. They interpreted this finding as supportive of a fractionation of remote memory but acknowledged that a longer follow up may be required in order to show a decline in autobiographical memory. Our study included a 2- to 3-year follow-up period, which was sufficient to demonstrate deficits in memory for public people and events and autobiographical memory. We found no significant correlation between the magnitude of decline on memory for public people and events and autobiographical memory, supporting Greene and Hodges’ hypothesis of separation of public and autobiographical memory.

The pattern of public memory deficits in the Greene and Hodges study was compatible with a loss of storage of memory traces, with retrieval deficits making a minor contribution to the memory impairment. We found no task (i.e., free recall versus recognition) × time interaction, demonstrating a similar decline for both conditions and suggesting that storage and retrieval abilities for public memory have a parallel decline in AD. Given the lack of significant decline in autobiographical memory in the Greene and Hodges study, the presence of a relative impairment of access and storage for autobiographical material could not be determined. We found a nonsignificant task × time interaction for autobiographical memory, suggesting a similar decline for access and storage of autobiographical memories.

The presence of a temporal gradient for public remote memory, namely a better recollection of early versus late memories, has been reported by some68 but not all studies.9 Nestor et al.10 recently suggested that the presence of a significant gradient may depend on the stage of illness and sample size. In our baseline study of remote memory, we could not find a temporal gradient for the recall of famous people and well-known events.2 However, the present follow-up study demonstrated a significant decade × time interaction, which resulted from a faster decline in the recall of recent as compared to earlier decades for the follow-up assessment only. While supporting the presence of a temporal gradient for public remote memory in AD, our study suggests that this phenomenon is absent in the early stages of the illness.

Fama et al.11 found significant associations between poor performance on the recognition section of a public remote memory test and smaller parieto-occipital cortical volumes and between poor anterograde (but not autobiographical) memory and smaller hippocampi volumes, suggesting a dissociation between cortically mediated remote memory and hippocampally mediated anterograde memory. Fama et al. also reported a dissociation between anterograde and retrograde memory performance in AD as well as no significant association between the severity of dementia and remote memory deficits.12 Our initial study demonstrated no significant association between deficits on a test of verbal anterograde memory and deficits on tests of autobiographical and public remote memory.2 However, we found a significant correlation between the magnitude of decline in anterograde verbal memory and in autobiographical memory. Taken together, these findings suggest the disruption of independent mechanisms of anterograde and autobiographical memory deficits early in AD but a common final pathway at later stages of the illness.

There is consistent evidence that anterograde memory deficits in AD result from medial temporal lobe pathology, but the pathological basis of autobiographical amnesia is less well known. A recent study by Maguire and Frith13 used functional MRI to examine brain activations during the retrieval of real life autobiographical event memories in young and elderly healthy adults. They found significant unilateral hippocampal activation in young adults but bilateral hippocampal activations in older adults. Future studies should examine whether both anterograde and autobiographical memory deficits result from neuropathological changes in different but close brain regions (e.g., medial and polar temporal regions, respectively) and whether extension of those changes may produce a parallel long-term decline in different memory domains.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Cecilia Tiberti and Flavia Dorrego for the memory assessments.

This study was supported by a joint grant from the Institutes Nationales de la Sante et Recherches Medicales (France) and the Consejo Nacional de Investigacion, Ciencia y Tecnica (Argentina).

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1.  Performance of the AD Group on the Free Recall Section of the Autobiographical Memory Scale Both at the Initial and Follow-Up Evaluations

AD=Alzheimer’s disease

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2.  Performance of the AD Group on the Free Recall Section of the Remote Memory Scale at the Initial and Follow-Up Evaluations

FIGURE 3.

FIGURE 3.  Significant Correlation Between Autobiographical Memory Scale Scores and the Buschke Selective Reminding Test Scores

TABLE 1. Autobiographical Memory Scale Scores at Baseline and at Follow Up
TABLE 2. Remote Memory Scale Scores at Baseline and at Follow Up

Received September 9, 2003; revised April 23, 2004; accepted May 7, 2004. From the School of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, University of Western Australia and Fremantle Hospital, Fremantle, Australia; the Buenos Aires Neuropsychiatric Center, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unité 324 INSERM, Paris. Address correspondence to Dr. Starkstein, Fremantle Hospital, Education Building T-7, Fremantle WA 6959, Australia; (E-mail).
References

1 Greene JD, Hodges JR: The fractionation of remote memory. Evidence from a longitudinal study of dementia of Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 1996; 34:537–551Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2 Dorrego MF, Sabe L, Garcia Cuerva A: Remote memory in Alzheimer’s disease Neurol 1999; 52:1403–147Google Scholar

3 McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurol 1984; 34:939–944Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Hachinski VC, Lassen NA, Marshall J: Multi-infarct dementia A cause of mental deterioration in the elderly Lancet 1974; 2:207–210Google Scholar

5 Buschke H, Fuld PA: Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurol 1974; 24:1019–1025Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Beatty WW, Salmon DP, Butters N, et al: Retrograde amnesia in patients with Alzheimer’s disease or Huntington’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging 1988; 9: 181–186Google Scholar

7 Hodges JR, McCarthy RA: Autobiographical amnesia resulting from bilateral paramedian thalamic infarction. A case study in cognitive neurobiology. Brain 1993; 116:921–940Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8 Greene JD, JR Hodges, Baddeley DA: Autobiographical memory and executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type Neuropsychologia 1995; 33:1647–1670Google Scholar

9 Wilson RS, Kazniak AW, Fox JH: Remote memory in senile dementia. Cortex 1981; 17:41–48Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10 Nestor PJ, Graham KS, Bozeat S, et al: Memory consolidation and the hippocampus: further evidence from studies of autobiographical memory in semantic dementia and frontal variant frontotemporal dementia Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:633–654Google Scholar

11 Fama R, Shear PK, Marsh L, et al: Remote memory for public figures in Alzheimer’s disease: relationships to regional cortical and limbic brain volumes J Int Neuropsychological Society 2001; 7:384–390Google Scholar

12 Fama R, Sullivan EV, Shear PK, et al: Extent, pattern, and correlates of remote memory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 2000; 14:265–76Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13 Maguire EA, Frith CD: Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain 2003; 126:1511–1523Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar