The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

To the Editor: Autism is a biological disorder with a clinical onset in the first years of life that persists, to varying degrees, throughout the lifetime. It is characterized by abnormality in reciprocal social interaction, communication, and language development as well as by repetitive and stereotyped behavior. These abnormalities are caused by defects in multiple areas of the brain. Although intense effort has been invested in discovering a physical cause for the etiology of autism, no one has yet succeeded. We are still a long way from claiming the knowledge of causality. 1

Reduced generalization has been noted in autism for many decades. 2 This key feature of people with autism can also be viewed from the perspective of artificial neural networks.

Artificial neural networks have been around since the seminal papers of McCulloch and Pitts 3 and Rosenblatt. 4 Many neural network models have been proposed and investigated. Artificial neural networks are built by using simplified models of biological neurons. The pattern of connections between the neural units is most often established via learning rules and gives the network its ability to compute complex functions and to develop emergent behaviors.

One problem that occurs during neural network training is overfitting—the error on the training set is driven to a very small value, but when new data are presented to the network, the error is large. In other words, the network has memorized the training examples, but it has not learned to generalize and find the true answer in new situations.

This overfitting in artificial neural networks is, to some extent, similar to what happens in the biological neural networks of the brain in people with autism. In other words, the interest that autistic persons show toward lawful and highly predictable systems 5 produces a situation that is analogous to overfitting. When overfitting happens in artificial neural networks, the network learns precisely the relationship between inputs and outputs in the training set, but it does not have the ability to adapt to novel circumstances. Consequently the generalization ability of the network is poor.

A memorizing procedure rather than learning happens in the brain of an autistic patient. Therefore, such a patient is not capable of incorporating prior knowledge in the learning process. This is in accordance with change resistance, need for sameness, and low generalization features of autistic persons.

Taking this similarity between artificial neural networks and autism into consideration may lead to novel learning procedures for these patients. For example, increasing the training set (i.e., the different situations to which the patient is exposed) and/or early stopping (i.e., reducing the intense concentration on training experiences) may be proper strategies in managing autistic patients.

Neuromuscular Systems Laboratory, Amirkabir University of Technology, Tehran, Iran
References

1. Cashin A, Waters C: The undervalued role of over-regulation in autism: chaos theory as a metaphor and beyond. J Child Adolesc Psychiatr Nurs 2006; 19:224–230Google Scholar

2. Rimland B: Infantile Autism: The Syndrome and its Implications for a Neural Theory of Behavior. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1964Google Scholar

3. McCulloch WS, Pitts W: A logical calculus of ideas immanent in nervous activity. Bull Math Biophys 1943; 5:115–133Google Scholar

4. Rosenblatt F: The perceptron: a probabilistic model for information storage and organization in the brain. Psychol Rev 1958; 65:386–408Google Scholar

5. Baron-Cohen S: The hyper-systemizing, assortative mating theory of autism. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2006; 30:865–872Google Scholar