The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
LetterFull Access

Visual Hallucinations in Alzheimer's Disease: Possible Involvement of Low Visual Acuity and Dementia With Lewy Bodies

Published Online:

SIR: I read with interest the article by Holroyd et al.1 where they showed that Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients with visual hallucinations had a significantly smaller occipital/whole brain ratio than AD patients without visual hallucinations. Although they mentioned limitations to their study such as small sample size, there are another two limitations as follows.

First, low visual acuity may be associated with visual hallucinations in AD patients. For example, Chapman et al.2 have reported that only impaired visual acuity was entered into the equation when logistic regression analysis examined cognition, visual acuity, and gender as associates of visual hallucinations in AD patients. Accordingly, it is necessary to compare visual acuity between AD patients with visual hallucinations and those without them in the patients of Holroyd et al.1 If the two groups had comparable visual acuity, their visual hallucinations might have derived from neuropathology of the occipital lobe. However, if AD patients with visual hallucinations had significantly lower visual acuity, it is uncertain whether their visual hallucinations derived from low visual acuity or from neuropathology of the occipital lobe or both. Actually, even psychologically normal old people can suffer from visual hallucinations, which is called Charles Bonnet syndrome.3,4

Second, although Holroyd et al.1 diagnosed probable AD according to NINCDS-ADRDA criteria, there is a possibility that their patients included patients suffering from dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) because AD+DLB patients are not easily distinguished clinically from pure AD cases.5 Moreover, visual hallucinations occur at significantly higher rates in DLB patients than in AD patients.5,6 If Holroyd and colleagues'1 group of probable AD patients with visual hallucinations actually included more DLB patients than their AD group without visual hallucinations, DLB rather than occipital atrophy might have been associated with visual hallucinations.

References

1 Holroyd S, Shepherd ML, Downs JH: Occipital atrophy is associated with visual hallucinations in Alzheimer's disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2000; 12:25–28LinkGoogle Scholar

2 Chapman FM, Dickinson J, McKeith I, et al: Association among visual hallucinations, visual actuity, and specific eye pathologies in Alzheimer's disease: treatment implications. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1983–1985Google Scholar

3 Teunisse RJ, Cruysberg JR, Hoefnagels WH, et al: Visual hallucinations in psychologically normal people: Charles Bonnet's syndrome. Lancet 1996; 347:794–797Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

4 Terao T: Effect of carbamazepine and clonazepam combination on Charles Bonnet syndrome: a case report. Hum Psychopharmacol Clin Exp 1998; 13:451–453CrossrefGoogle Scholar

5 Luis CA, Barker WW, Cajaraj K, et al: Sensitivity and specificity of three clinical criteria for dementia with Lewy bodies in an autopsy-verified sample. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999; 14:526–533Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6 Ballard C, Holmes C, McKeith I, et al: Psychiatric morbidity in dementia with Lewy bodies: a prospective clinical and neuropathological comparative study with Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1039–1045Google Scholar