The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×
Published Online:

I nfection with HIV often leads to neuropathology, which is diffusely distributed throughout brain parenchyma but is commonly believed to preferentially disrupt the structure and function of frontostriatal and temporolimbic systems. 2 Understandably, the vast majority of neuropsychological research in HIV has therefore been centered on constructs of direct relevance to these neural systems (e.g., executive functions, working memory, motor skills, and episodic memory). However, very little is currently known about the nature and extent of deficits in spatial cognition in persons living with HIV infection. Spatial cognition refers to the ability to detect, understand, manipulate, and integrate visual stimuli in the context of its environment. Although spatial cognition is primarily linked to the integrity of the posterior parietal cortex, optimally functioning parieto-striato-cortical pathways are also needed for the integration of visual input, 3 as observed in patients with frontostriatal pathology (e.g., Parkinson’s disease). 4 Considering that frontostriatal dysfunction is considered a hallmark of HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders, this overlap suggests that research regarding spatial cognition may in fact be relevant to the neuropsychology of HIV.

Early studies of spatial cognition in HIV included limited batteries and did not find much evidence of impairment, 5 , 6 leading to a temporary neglect of this thread of research. However, recent meta-analyses have produced mixed results regarding the effect to which spatial abilities are affected by HIV infection, 7 , 8 suggesting that there may be a signal worthy of further examination. Indeed, a closer review of the HIV literature explores the fronto-striatal components of this construct, indicating that subtler differences may exist in the distinct subcategories of spatial cognition (e.g., visuoconstruction, spatial attention) in this cohort. For instance, Poutiainen and colleagues 9 found significant differences between a group of 13 HIV+ individuals and 10 seronegative control subjects on measures of visuoconstruction, visuospatial skills, and praxis. Furthermore, Martin 10 suggested that HIV infection was associated with impairment on measures of egocentric spatial ability (i.e., spatial tasks that are built around the participant’s frame of reference, as opposed to that of an observer). Measures of spatial attention 11 , 12 and, more recently, number line orientation 13 also may be impaired in HIV infection, perhaps demonstrating the particular sensitivity of visuospatial tasks that draw upon complex attention and working memory to the prefrontostriatal pathologies of HIV disease.

Mental rotation, the ability to manipulate three-dimensional objects in space, is a widely studied and neurally complex area of spatial cognition that has been largely ignored in the neuro-AIDS literature. These tasks are sensitive to both the functioning of the posterior parietal cortex and fronto-striato-parietal neural networks. 14 While mental rotation has been studied more extensively in other frontostriatal disorders (i.e., Parkinson’s disease), 15 to our knowledge only one study to date has examined this construct in HIV infection. Olesen and colleagues 16 used a computerized mental rotation task in which two stimuli appear at different degrees of rotation; participants are asked to evaluate whether the objects are identical ( same ) or mirror images of each other ( different ). Impairment between groups presented as a disproportionate increase in errors and reaction time in the HIV+ group (n=14) as the degree of rotation increased. Although the reaction time analyses showed trend-level group differences overall, this effect was primarily seen in the object condition, as explained by a three-way interaction between stimulus, group, and level of rotation. In conjunction with group differences found on a measure of global versus locally biased processing, the authors concluded that HIV-related deficits in spatial cognition were likely due to parietal lobe pathology.

Although these data support the notion of impaired spatial cognition in HIV infection, the lack of supplemental neuroanatomical and neuropsychological data raises questions about the underlying neural and cognitive mechanisms. Despite relying on the posterior parietal cortex for basic processing of visual input, frontal systems are additionally required to adequately integrate, manipulate, and utilize visual information. 3 For instance, the ability to mentally rotate three-dimensional objects requires the external processes of viewing the object as it physically exists prerotation, but also simultaneously viewing and manipulating it on the visuospatial scratch pad of the mind’s eye. 17 Additionally, the cognitive requirements of the task likely vary based on the relative position of the object to be rotated. In fact, neuroimaging studies have shown that different types of rotations (i.e., object-centered or viewer-centered) rely on discrete neural pathways (i.e., right parietal lobes and intraparietal sulcus, or left parietal and left frontal regions, respectively). 18 The involvement of frontal regions and the internal manipulation requirements of the task imply that mental rotation not only requires intact visuospatial skills but also optimally functioning working memory abilities. Considering the overlapping neural networks between mental rotation abilities and HIV-associated neuropathologies, further exploration is needed to determine the neuropsychological correlates of this impairment, thereby elucidating the deficient component processes. In the present study, two tasks of mental rotation were administered within a larger battery containing measures of executive functions, information processing, motor skills, as well as other tasks of spatial cognition. We hypothesized that there would be evidence of HIV-related impairment on tasks of mental rotation and that poorer performance would be associated with impairment on measures of executive functions and working memory. Furthermore, we hypothesized that mental rotation impairments would be most evident on the hand rotation task, as egocentric spatial tasks tend to rely more heavily on frontal systems, 19 which are particularly affected by HIV infection.

METHODS

Participants

The study was approved by the University Human Research Protections Program, and all participants provided written, informed consent. All participants were recruited from a larger cohort at the HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) who agreed to participate in an additional hour of neuropsychological testing that was funded by a Core Support Program for AIDS Research (CSPAR) developmental grant designed to examine spatial cognition in HIV. Participants included 19 individuals with HIV infection (HIV+) and 15 HIV seronegative comparison subjects (HIV−). HIV serostatus was determined by enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays and a Western Blot confirmatory test. Potential participants were prescreened for histories of exclusionary psychiatric (e.g., mental retardation, psychotic disorders) or neurological (e.g., seizure disorders, traumatic brain injury, cerebrovascular disease) disorders by a subject recruiter. Upon enrollment, participants were subsequently further screened by the study psychometrist using an unpublished, but standardized and semistructured, interview. Additionally, participants who tested positive for illicit drugs (except marijuana) on a urine toxicology screen conducted on the day of testing were excluded. The study groups did not differ statistically in age, education, sex, handedness, and ethnicity (p>0.10 in all cases; see Table 1 for demographic and disease characteristics). Premorbid verbal IQ, as measured by a demographically based estimate from the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, 20 was also comparable between groups (p>0.10). No between-group differences were observed on the Profile of Mood States, 21 which is a self-report measure of mood disturbance from the previous week (p>0.10).

TABLE 1. Demographic and HIV Disease Characteristics of the Study Samples
TABLE 1. Demographic and HIV Disease Characteristics of the Study Samples
Enlarge table

Materials and Procedure

Mental Rotation

Participants were administered two tasks of mental rotation (i.e., hands and parallelograms) 1 as part of an extensive neuropsychological, medical, and psychiatric evaluation. These particular mental rotation tasks were chosen because they were published paper-and-pencil tasks that were created by the same author (i.e., Luria 1 ) to maximize comparability. More importantly and directly related to our a priori hypotheses, together these tasks examine both ego- (i.e., body part) and allo-centric (i.e., object) mental rotation. For the hand rotation task (HIV+: n=19; HIV−: n=15), participants were presented with 21 1.5×2 cm line drawings of hands in different postures and orientations for which they were asked to indicate whether a right or left hand was depicted. The 10-item parallelogram task (HIV+: n=19; HIV−: n=13, due to minor administration errors) required individuals to examine the placement of a circle in a rotated parallelogram and subsequently draw circles onto two empty mirror-imaged parallelograms so that they would appear identical to the rotated figure (for further details, please see Luria 1 ). Performance on both tasks was recorded as total number of errors.

Standard Clinical Tests

The comprehensive battery was administered as part of the standard HNRC protocol to which this developmental grant was linked and included tests tapping standard cognitive domains that are (a) sensitive to HIV infection and (b) implicated in mental rotation performance, including executive functions, working memory, processing speed, and visuoperception. Specifically, individual tests included the following: Trail Making test (parts A and B, difference in time to completion [B−A]) 22 ; Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST-64, raw perseverative responses) 23 ; Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT-200, raw total correct) 24 ; Grooved Pegboard (time to completion) 25 ; Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed. (WAIS-III) Processing Speed Index (raw total score) 26 ; Hooper Visual Organization Test (raw total correct) 27 ; and Benton Judgment of Line Orientation (raw total correct). 27

Data Analyses

A repeated-measures analysis of variance with HIV serostatus as the between-groups variable and mental rotation task (i.e., hands or parallelograms) as the within-group variable was conducted to evaluate the primary study hypotheses. Follow-up analyses were conducted using independent-samples t tests and Cohen’s d effect size estimates. Note that, although the mental rotation data were non-normally distributed according to Shapiro-Wilk W tests (parallelograms: W=0.94, p<0.06; hands: W=0.93, p<0.05), follow-up analyses using a nonparametric approach yielded the same results; the parametric approach to testing the statistical interaction is described in addition for ease of interpretation. Nevertheless, correlational analyses regarding the associations between mental rotation and standard clinical tests within the HIV+ group were conducted using Spearman’s rho. Considering the small sample sizes, a critical alpha level of 0.05 was used throughout to minimize our risk of type II statistical error. To minimize our risk of type I error, correlations were analyzed only in the HIV+ group (rather than in both groups separately) and only when the mental rotation task differed by serostatus. This approach has been shown to (a) provide clear insight of neuropsychological constructs in a clinical sample (cf. convolution of shared variance in mixed clinical samples) and (b) reduce the risk of type I error by running correlational analyses only in the target population (cf. running analyses separately in the clinical and healthy comparison groups, which may then require Bonferroni-like corrections, in turn diluting genuinely significant findings). 28 , 29

RESULTS

A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed no main effects of HIV status (F=0.02, df=1, 29, p>0.10, η 2 =0.007) or mental rotation task (i.e., hands or parallelograms) (F=0.08, df=1, 29, p>0.10, η 2 =0.078). However, the interaction between group and task proved significant (F=5.47, df=1, 29, p=0.020, η 2 =0.168). Based on the non-normal distribution of the data, these statistics were also conducted using nonparametric analyses (i.e., Wilcoxon rank sum test), yielding a significant between-groups result on a hands minus parallelograms errors difference score variable (χ 2 =7.02, df=1, n=32, p=0.008). A planned follow-up univariate analysis indicated that the interaction effect was driven by a greater number of errors committed by the HIV+ group on the hands task (mean=6.5, SD=4.2) compared with the HIV− group (mean=3.9, SD=2.4; t 29 =2.27, p=0.031), representing a large effect (d=0.74). There was no significant difference between groups on the parallelograms task (t 27 =−0.89, p>0.10; d=−0.31) ( Figure 1 ).

FIGURE 1. Interaction Between Mental Rotation Tasks (Parallelograms and Hands) and HIV Serostatus

*p<0.05

To examine neuropsychological correlates of our significant primary finding, follow-up analyses were performed in the HIV+ group (n=19) to obtain Spearman’s rho correlation coefficients, as shown in Table 2 . Errors on the hands task were moderately associated with measures of working memory and executive functioning, as defined as poorer performance on the PASAT-200 and increased perseverations on WCST-64, respectively (p≤0.05 in both cases; see Figure 2 ). All other neuropsychological constructs examined (i.e., motor skills, information processing, divided attention, and visuospatial functioning) did not relate to mental rotation performance (p>0.10 in all cases). Additionally, performance on the hands task was not associated with HIV disease characteristics, including current and nadir CD4 count, plasma viral load, AIDS status, highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART), and estimated duration of infection (p>0.10 in all cases). Total mood disturbance as measured by the Profile of Mood States also did not relate to hand rotation errors (p>0.10).

TABLE 2. Correlations Between a Task of Mental Rotation (Hands) and Select Neuropsychological Tests in the HIV+ Sample (n=19)
TABLE 2. Correlations Between a Task of Mental Rotation (Hands) and Select Neuropsychological Tests in the HIV+ Sample (n=19)
Enlarge table
FIGURE 2. Relationship Between Mental Rotation of Hands and the PASAT-200 Total Correct and WCST-64 Perseverative Responses

p≤0.05 in all cases

DISCUSSION

Overall, our findings revealed that HIV infection is associated with impairment in mental rotation. As predicted, a significant interaction was observed between HIV serostatus and mental rotation task, indicating that individuals with HIV committed a greater number of errors than their seronegative counterparts on Luria’s hand rotation task, but not on the corresponding parallelogram rotation task. In other words, persons infected with HIV experienced disproportionately greater difficulties accurately manipulating mental representations of hands. This finding, which was associated with a large effect size, contrasts against the widely held belief that deficits in spatial cognition are not generally evident in individuals with HIV; in fact, these preliminary data build on a small literature suggesting that HIV infection may lead to impairment in complex spatial cognition, 7 , 10 , 13 including one prior study on mental rotation deficits. 16

Although this pilot study contributes to the basic evidence of a mental rotation deficit in HIV, as was first reported by Olesen and colleagues, 16 it also argues for an alternate interpretation of the underlying cognitive (and neural) substrates of this deficit. As noted above, Olesen et al. 16 attributed the HIV-associated mental rotation deficit to an impairment in visuospatial processing related to dysregulation of posterior parietal cortex; however, findings from the current study indicate that fronto-striatal systems may also contribute to the observed deficits. First, we observed a disproportionate impairment in the mental rotation of body parts (i.e., hands) as compared to objects (i.e., parallelograms). Although we did not directly query participants as to their relative strategies for mental rotation, it is posited that the mental rotation of body parts (relative to object rotation) requires greater integration of visuoperceptual and motor neural systems, whereby viewers imagine rotating their own extremities in relation to the stimuli. 19 These viewer-centered mental processes during body part rotation are linked to traditional spatial cognition circuits (e.g., posterior parietal cortex), 30 as well as fronto-striato-thalamo-cortical loops. 15 , 18 , 19 This interpretation also converges with a prior study by Martin, 10 who found that HIV infection was associated with deficits in egocentric spatial ability.

In further support of the involvement of frontal systems, deficits in the mental rotation of hands were associated with executive and working memory dysfunction in the HIV+ group. More specifically, errors on the hand rotation task demonstrated medium-to-large correlations in the expected directions with performance on the PASAT-200 and perseverative responses on the WCST-64. In contrast, hand rotation errors did not relate to performance on tasks of visuospatial functioning, information processing speed, or motor skills. Although the PASAT-200 also places demands on processing speed, the lack of relationship between mental rotation and the WAIS-III Processing Speed Index supports the hypothesis that the correspondence between the PASAT-200 and hands may be primarily driven by working memory. Taken together, these data provide convergent and divergent evidence that the HIV-associated deficit in mental rotation of hands involves cognitive processes that are strongly linked to frontal systems, which are preferentially affected in HIV disease. 2 , 7 Nevertheless, it is likely that this deficit arises from dysfunction in a distributed fronto-striato-parietal network that includes the posterior parietal cortex, which is involved in general mental rotation, 31 as well as in working memory 32 and executive functions. 33

Although our interpretation of the cognitive and neural mechanisms of HIV-associated mental rotation deficits departs from that of Olesen et al., 16 who emphasized the role of visuoperception and the parietal cortex, a few important methodological differences exist between these two studies. First, Olesen et al. 16 used computerized mental rotation tasks that included reaction time, which was not captured by our paper-and-pencil tasks. Second, their tasks employed a same/different response paradigm in which two hands were shown, whereas the task used in this study required a right/left response to a single stimuli. Additionally, our object rotation task used two-dimensional parallelograms, compared to the traditional—and arguably more difficult—three-dimensional blocks in Olesen et al.’s study. Indeed, Olesen et al.’s 16 findings may be interpreted as a breakdown in spatial working memory, thus supporting the possible involvement of frontal systems, which the authors themselves acknowledged as an alternate interpretation. A more direct evaluation of the role of fronto-striato-parietal systems in HIV-associated mental rotation impairment is needed, perhaps using experimental paradigms to manipulate the cognitive components of rotation in relation to structural (e.g., morphological) and functional (e.g., metabolic and neural) neuroimaging markers.

Results from this study are not confounded by demographic factors or affective distress, as the HIV+ and seronegative groups were well balanced on these characteristics. However, the small sample size (total N=34) and well-controlled disease status of the HIV+ group likely limited our power to detect more modest relationships between hand rotation and other cognitive ability areas (e.g., visuoperception) and disease parameters. Indeed, rotation errors were not significantly associated with any HIV disease or treatment factors (e.g., HAART status). Despite the relatively small sample sizes, the present study was reasonably well-powered to detect at least a medium effect size interaction using a critical alpha of 0.05 (1−β=0.78). To minimize risk of type II error (given our small sample size), we did not use additional corrective measures for multiple comparisons (e.g., Bonferroni’s corrections), but instead employed other methodological techniques (e.g., conducting analyses only in the HIV sample) to control type I error while still using the standard critical alpha of 0.05. It is, however, important to note that these results would not be statistically significant had we controlled for multiple comparisons. Moreover, these limitations likely restrict the extent to which our results are generalizable to the broader HIV-infected population. Nevertheless, the presence of a significant HIV-associated impairment in hand rotation and its correspondence to deficits in working memory and executive functions in such a small HIV sample with relatively well-controlled disease speaks to the magnitude of the observed effect. Another limitation of the present findings is that the mental rotation tasks were not specifically constructed for the purpose of dissociation and therefore differed in several respects, including length and response modality (i.e., the parallelogram task required a motor response). As such, studies are needed regarding the psychometric characteristics of these mental rotation tasks (e.g., reliability and demographically adjusted normative standards). Future studies are needed to examine the relationship between mental rotation and its cognitive mechanisms across a larger group of persons with HIV infection with a broader range of disease characteristics. Further examination of spatial cognition in HIV may also lead to enhancements in the detection of problems in everyday functioning (e.g., driving), as has been shown in older adults 34 and clinical populations, including Parkinson’s disease patients. 35

Received April 26, 2008; revised November 26, 2008 and February 4, 2009; accepted March 13, 2009. The authors are affiliated with the Department of Psychiatry at the University of California, San Diego. Address correspondence to Steven Paul Woods, Psy.D., Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 220 Dickinson Street, Suite B, Mail Code 8231, San Diego, CA 92103; [email protected] (e-mail).

Copyright © 2010 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

References

1. Luria AR: Higher Cortical Functions in Man. New York, Basic Books, 1966Google Scholar

2. Gonzalez-Scarano F, Martin-Garcia J: The neuropathogenesis of AIDS. Nat Rev Immunol 2005; 5:69–81Google Scholar

3. Lawrence AD, Watkins LHA, Sahakian BJ, et al: Visual object and visuospatial cognition in Huntington’s disease: implications for information processing in corticostriatal circuits. Brain 2000; 123:1349–1364Google Scholar

4. Boller F, Passafiume D, Keefe NC, et al: Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson’s disease: role of perceptual and motor factors. Arch Neurology 1984; 41:485–490Google Scholar

5. Heaton RK, Grant I, Butters N, et al: The HNRC 500–neuropsychology of HIV infection at different disease stages. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1995; 1:231–251Google Scholar

6. Saykin AJ, Janssen RS, Sprehn GC, et al: Neuropsychological dysfunction in HIV-infection: characterization in a lymphadenopathy cohort. Int J Clin Neuropsychology 1988; 10:81–95Google Scholar

7. Reger M, Welsh R, Razani J, et al: A meta-analysis of the neuropsychological sequelae of HIV infection. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002; 8:410–424Google Scholar

8. Cysique LA, Maruff P, Brew BJ: The neuropsychological profile of symptomatic AIDS and ADC patients in the pre-HAART era: a meta-analysis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006; 12:368–382Google Scholar

9. Poutiainen E, Iivanainen M, Elovaara I, et al: Cognitive changes as early signs of HIV infection. Acta Neurologica 1988; 78:49–52Google Scholar

10. Martin A: HIV, cognition, and the basal ganglia, in Neuropsychology of HIV Infection. Edited by Grant I, Martin A. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994Google Scholar

11. Martin EM, Sorensen DJ, Robertson LC, et al: Spatial attention in HIV-1 infection: a preliminary report. J Neuropsychiatry 1992; 4:288–293Google Scholar

12. Maruff P, Malone V, McArthur-Jackson C, et al: Abnormalities of visual spatial attention in HIV infection and the HIV-associated dementia complex. J Neuropsychiatry 1995; 7:325–333Google Scholar

13. Bogdanova Y, Neargarder S, Cronin-Golomb A: Mapping mental number line in physical space: vertical and horizontal visual number line orientation in asymptomatic individuals with HIV. Neuropsychologia 2008; 46:2914–2923Google Scholar

14. Shepherd RN, Metzler J: Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 1971; 171:701–703Google Scholar

15. Amick MM, Schendan HE, Ganis G, et al: Frontostriatal circuits are necessary for visuomotor transformation: mental rotation in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44:339–349Google Scholar

16. Olesen PJ, Schendan HE, Amick MM, et al: HIV infection affects parietal-dependent spatial cognition: evidence from mental rotation and hierarchical pattern perception. Behav Neurosci 2007; 121:1163–1173Google Scholar

17. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ: Working memory, in Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation, vol 8. Edited by Bower G. New York, Academic Press, 1974Google Scholar

18. Kosslyn SM, DiGirolamo GJ, Thompson WL, et al: Mental rotation of objects versus hands: neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology 1998; 35:151–161Google Scholar

19. Parsons LM, Fox PT, Downs JH, et al: Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature 1995; 375:54–58Google Scholar

20. Psychological Corporation: Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 2001Google Scholar

21. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF: Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego, Calif, Educational and Industrial Testing Service, 1981Google Scholar

22. Reitan RM, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Tucson, Ariz, Neuropsychology Press, 1985Google Scholar

23. Kongs SK, Thompson LL, Iverson GL, et al: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–64 card computerized version. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 2000Google Scholar

24. Diehr MC, Heaton RK, Miller W, et al: The Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT): norms for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment 1998; 5:375–387Google Scholar

25. Kløve H: Grooved Pegboard. Lafayette, Ind, Lafayette Instruments, 1963Google Scholar

26. Psychological Corporation: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1997Google Scholar

27. Benton AL, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, et al: Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994Google Scholar

28. Delis DC, Jacobson M, Bondi MW, et al: The myth of testing construct validity using factor analysis or correlations with normal or mixed clinical populations: lessons from memory assessment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003; 9:936–946Google Scholar

29. Brandt J: 2005 INS Presidential Address: neuropsychological crimes and misdemeanors. Clin Neuropsychol 2007; 21:553–568Google Scholar

30. Vingerhoets G, de Lange FP, Vandemaele P, et al: Motor imagery in mental rotation: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2002; 17:1623–1633Google Scholar

31. Zacks JM: Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: a meta-analysis and review. J Cogn Neuroscience 2008; 20:1–19Google Scholar

32. Lazeron RH, Rombouts SA, de Sonneville L, et al: A paced visual serial addition task for fMRI. J Neurol Sciences 2003; 213:29–34Google Scholar

33. Buchsbaum BR, Greer S, Chang WL, et al: Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin Card-Sorting Task and component processes. Hum Brain Mapp 2005; 25:35–45Google Scholar

34. Richardson ED, Nadler JD, Malloy PF: Neuropsychologic prediction of performance measures of daily living skills in geriatric patients. Neuropsychology 1995; 9:565–572Google Scholar

35. Amick MM, Grace J, Ott BR: Visual and cognitive predictors of driving safety in Parkinson’s disease patients. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2007; 22:957–967Google Scholar